cover of episode Dr. Mark D'Esposito: How to Optimize Cognitive Function & Brain Health

Dr. Mark D'Esposito: How to Optimize Cognitive Function & Brain Health

2024/2/19
logo of podcast Huberman Lab

Huberman Lab

Chapters

El Dr. Mark D'Esposito, neurólogo y profesor de neurociencia y psicología en la Universidad de California, Berkeley, habla sobre los mecanismos cerebrales que subyacen a la cognición y las formas de memoria necesarias para la concentración, la productividad, la planificación y el logro de objetivos, y el aprendizaje.
  • La función ejecutiva es fundamental para todos los aspectos de nuestra vida diaria.
  • No hay separación entre la función ejecutiva y la memoria.
  • Este episodio proporciona una comprensión moderna de la cognición y la memoria, y herramientas prácticas para optimizar la salud y la función del cerebro.

Shownotes Transcript

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. My guest today is Dr. and Professor Mark Desposito. Dr. Mark Desposito is a neurologist and a professor of neuroscience and psychology at the University of California, Berkeley. He is a world expert in the brain mechanisms controlling executive function and memory.

Executive function is the way in which we are able to designate and carry out specific cognitive strategies. And it is fundamental to every aspect of our daily lives. And because so much of being effective in daily life

involves using specific context relevant batches of information in order to understand what to do and when, and what not to do and when, and to come up with strategies that are very adaptive for us to move forward in the context of relationships, work, school, and athletics, and on and on.

There's really no separation between executive function and memory. And today, Dr. Desposito explains the neural circuits controlling executive function and memory, how they interact, the key role of dopamine in executive function and something called working memory, and teaches us ways to optimize executive function and memory, that is, how to optimize cognitive function. In addition to discussing how to optimize cognitive function in the healthy brain, today

Today's discussion also centers around how to restore cognitive function in disease or injury conditions that deplete executive function and memory, such as traumatic brain injury, concussion, Alzheimer's, Parkinson's, and attention deficit disorders. Dr. Desposito shares with us research findings both about behavioral and pharmacologic strategies to enhance executive function and memory.

By the end of today's discussion, you will have learned from Dr. Desposito a tremendous amount about the modern understanding of cognition, that is thinking and memory, and the carrying out of specific cognitive strategies. You will also learn a tremendous amount about how to optimize brain function and brain health. Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is Maui Nui Venison. Maui Nui Venison is the most nutrient-dense and delicious red meat available. I've spoken before on this podcast, and there's general consensus that most people should strive to consume approximately one gram of protein per pound of body weight.

Now, when one strives to do that, it's important to maximize the quality of that protein intake to the calorie ratio because you don't want to consume an excess of calories when trying to get that one gram of protein per pound of body weight. Maui Nui venison has an extremely high quality protein to calorie ratio, so it makes getting that one gram of protein per pound of body weight extremely easy. It's also delicious. Personally, I like the ground venison. I also like the venison steaks.

And then for convenience, when I'm on the road, I like the jerky. The jerky is a very high protein to calorie ratio. So it has as much as 10 grams of protein per jerky stick. And it has something like only like 55 calories. So again, making it very easy to get enough protein without consuming excess calories. If you would like to try Maui Nui venison, you can go to mauinuivenison.com slash Huberman to get 20% off your first order. Again, that's mauinuivenison.com slash Huberman to get 20% off.

Today's episode is also brought to us by Juve. Juve makes medical grade red light therapy devices. Now, if there's one thing I've consistently emphasized on this podcast, it's the incredible role that light can have on our biology. And of course, I'm always telling people that they should get sunlight in their eyes as soon as possible after waking on as many days of their life as possible for sake of setting circadian rhythm, daytime mood focus and alertness and improve sleep.

Now, in addition to sunlight, red light and near infrared light has been shown to have positive effects on improving numerous aspects of cellular and organ health.

including faster muscle recovery, improved skin health and wound healing, even improvements in acne, or that is removal of acne, reducing pain and inflammation, improving mitochondrial function, and even improving vision itself. What sets Juve apart and why it's my preferred red light therapy device is that it has clinically proven wavelengths, meaning it uses specific wavelengths of red light and near infrared light in combination that trigger the optimal cellular adaptations.

Personally, I use the handheld juve every day. The handheld juve is about the size of a thick piece of toast.

And I also own a Juve panel that allows for full body exposure. And I use that one approximately five times per week for about 10 to 15 minutes per session. If you would like to try Juve, you can go to juve.com slash Huberman to receive $50 off your first purchase. Again, that's Juve, spelled J-O-O-V-E.com slash Huberman to get $50 off your first purchase. Today's episode is also brought to us by Eight Sleep.

Eight Sleep makes smart mattress covers with cooling, heating, and sleep tracking capacity. I've spoken many times before in this podcast about the fact that sleep is the foundation of mental health, physical health, and performance. Now, a key component of getting a great night's sleep

is that in order to fall and stay deeply asleep, your body temperature actually has to drop by about one to three degrees. And in order to wake up feeling refreshed and energized, your body temperature actually has to increase by about one to three degrees. One of the best ways to make sure that those temperature changes occur at the appropriate times, at the beginning and throughout, and at the end of your night when you wake up, is to control the temperature of your sleeping environment. And that's what eight sleep allows you to do.

It allows you to program the temperature of your mattress and sleeping environment such that you fall and stay deeply asleep easily and wake up each morning feeling incredibly refreshed and energized.

I've been sleeping on an Eight Sleep mattress cover for almost three years now, and it has dramatically improved the quality of my sleep. If you'd like to try Eight Sleep, you can go to eightsleep.com slash Huberman to get $150 off their pod three mattress cover. Eight Sleep currently ships in the USA, Canada, UK, select countries in the EU and Australia. Again, that's eightsleep.com slash Huberman. And now for my discussion with Dr. Mark Desposito. Dr. Desposito, welcome.

Hey, Andrew. Thank you so much for inviting me. I'm really looking forward to our conversation. You may not remember me, but I remember you when I was a first-year graduate student and you showed up at Berkeley, one of the first people to really bring functional imaging of the human brain to Berkeley, bring a neurology and a clinical...

emphasis to the neuroscience studies there and it's really just blossomed and it's been a real thrill for me to see all the magnificent work out of your laboratory over the years and I know you also still see patients. So the topics that are of interest to you I know are of great interest to our audience. Maybe we'll just start off with a few of the basics and do a little functional neuroanatomy lesson for folks. Not to scare anyone, don't worry. This will be accessible to everyone. And just talk about

frontal lobes and prefrontal cortex and a little bit of what those structures do. Because many times on this podcast, I've said, okay, the neural real estate right behind your forehead is involved in context and planning, et cetera, but you're the real expert here. How should we think about what the frontal lobes do and their various roles in health and disease?

Yeah, so there's four lobes. There's the frontal lobes, parietal, temporal, occipital, and the frontal lobes probably take up more, do take up more territory than the other lobes, probably about a third of the cortex. And within the frontal lobes, I don't, I'm going to use sort of frontal lobes probably in our conversation a lot, but what I really mean is the prefrontal cortex. So within the frontal lobes, there's also areas that are important for motor function as well.

But when we're talking about the frontal lobes and talking about its, you know, involvement in higher level cognitive abilities, we're talking about the prefrontal cortex. And this is what's considered sort of the highest level of cortex in the brain. So, yeah, when you think about it, people assign it all sorts of functions. Almost every function you think of, people have sort of put into the frontal lobes. But I think what we've all kind of...

move towards is this idea of executive function, this ability to plan, to organize, to really transfer our thoughts into an action and really to be guided by goals and intentions and not be kind of ruled by sort of just automatic behaviors. A word we use in cognitive neuroscience is called cognitive control.

Cog control, executive function is what we attribute to the frontal lobe. And so you can think of it as, you know, the CEO of the brain or the conductor of the orchestra, really the part of the brain that's really controlling the rest of the brain. So, yeah, if you had to choose which part you wanted to not leave home, it's your frontal lobes. Speaking of which, what are some of the symptoms of mild frontal lobe damage?

And severe frontal lobe damage, damage brought about either through neurodegenerative disease or physical injury. I know we're going to talk a bit about both today or a lot about both. But how would lack of executive function show up maybe on kind of a subtle level?

Yeah, I mean, first I should say is that it shows up all the time because when-- and frontal lobe behavior is probably much more prevalent than we realize. Certainly we think about it when you have a brain injury to the frontal lobes and there's lots of neurological disorders like stroke and traumatic brain injury and Alzheimer's disease that can affect the frontal lobe and there's a number of, you know, psychiatric disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder and schizophrenia and depression that are thought to be frontal lobe dysfunction.

But when you're sleep deprived and when you're stressed and just normal aging, the frontal lobe seems to be the first system that's affected because it really is involved in the highest level. So, when we're having a bad day, when we're having difficulty sort of setting priorities, when we're having difficulties achieving the goal that we've set out, when we get distracted,

When we're not able to sort of adapt and be flexible, these are all the type of things that reflect that our frontal lobes are not functioning optimally. Approximately what age does the frontal lobe circuitry come online, so to speak? I mean, when I see a baby, babies can orient their eyes towards things, but they're rather reflexive in where they'll place their eyes. But by the time kids are three or four, they can certainly play with blocks or interact with other children or their parents.

but it seems that, you know, full functionality of the frontal lobes is really gradual. At least that's my non-clinically trained assessment, you know. Yeah, I mean, it's a really tough question to know when they're fully developed because these studies haven't been done. When MRI was introduced and we were able to sort of image the brain in a non-invasive way, then studies did start to come out trying to sort of map out at what age does your frontal lobes fully develop.

And it seemed like it was early into your 20s. You know, I always say that it's not surprising that you can't rent a car until you're 25. That the insurance companies knew before. Neuroscientists did as to when your frontal lobes have, you know, when your decision-making skills

at their highest. And so that's probably about right into your 20s is probably before your frontal lobes are fully developed. And it's really interesting question is why does it take so long? It's the area of the brain that takes the longest to develop. And why is that? And I

I think there's a reason. I think that this sort of slow development of frontal lobes allows us to explore, allows us to think about novel ways of solving problems, allows us to take in the world. If they were shut off earlier, it would lead to maybe a much more sort of rigid, kind of less flexible kind of...

that we've seen things. So I think that it helps to take a long time to develop, but also it obviously leads to some problems sometimes in adolescence, as we see sometimes.

Can one see a lack of frontal lobe maturity in just the sheer number of physical movements that a child makes? So for instance, in a classroom of, you know, let's say, you know, fourth graders, oftentimes there'll be a range of

apparent ability of kids to sit still or to listen? Do we think that the kid that's having a hard time focusing and listening to instructions or studying their body when they're told to sit still, I don't know if they still tell kids to sit still, but they were telling me to sit still when I was a kid. Is that somehow reflective of a, you know, slightly lagging frontal frontal lobe function and maturity? Whereas that, you know, the kids that can sit

you know, still and stoic and focus, does that mean that they're a little bit more accelerated along that trajectory? Yeah, it's hard to say. I mean, the frontal lobe is a big territory and we can get into it, but there's, you know, the frontal lobe probably has 25 different sub-regions within it. And so grossly, we think about the frontal lobes as the lateral portion of the frontal lobes.

which is involved in these executive function, probably supports these executive function abilities. But then we've got another part of the frontal lobes called the orbital frontal cortex, which is probably involved more in social and emotional behavior. So, you know, when we think of, again, when we think about frontal lobe behaviors, they kind of, you have to break, there's so many different types of frontal lobe behaviors. So that type of behavior, which may be involved in sort of being able to inhibit, you know,

your motor movements or maybe not being distracted may reflect that that system is a little bit delayed, but it could be that another system, the one that's involved in planning and organizes, you know, is more developed. And I do think they develop at different trajectories. So with the frontal lobes essentially serving an executive or CEO type function, goal-directed behavior, intentions, cognitive control, these are the terms you used, right?

Where are the rules? What do the rules look like? You know, when I think about brain function, which I've spent a lot of my life thinking about, think about chemical and electrical signaling between neurons, different neurons communicating more or less at a given moment.

reflecting some sort of circuit as we call it. And then some behavior or some decision comes out. And if I, for instance, I have to get my driver's license renewed soon. So if I go to the Department of Motor Vehicles, what a lovely experience that is. The moment I get there, I sort of lock into a certain rule set. When I'm home, I'm in a different rule set. When I'm with my friends versus when I'm with my parents, different rule sets. And it seems that the frontal lobe is really good at

drawing on context based on knowledge of where one is, and then coming up with kind of algorithms that are appropriate or inappropriate to run in that context.

But what is the nature of these algorithms? Are they of the, okay, shut down all cursing in this environment. Okay, you're free to just quote unquote, be you. I mean, when it really comes down to it, it has some interesting philosophical aspects too, because just be yourself, be authentic, be vulnerable, you know, all these things make sense. But of course, one needs to be appropriate with the context.

So how does this work? What are the algorithms? How does this work? Right, because that's a pretty common example of our patients that they don't follow the rules. If you're sitting in someone's office and the phone rings, you know not to pick up his phone, but the patients don't and they may pick up the phone. There's this Dr. Lameet who's a

A neurologist from France published these beautiful papers in the 80s of all these things that patients did that just broke the rules and just kind of pulled to their environment without having any context to it. If he put a pair of glasses on the table and didn't ask them to put them on, they would put them on even if they had a pair of glasses on already. Or he took them to their apartment and they saw the bed and they jump into the bed and go under the covers.

He had a nurse, and he put a blood pressure cuff there, and she picked up the blood pressure cuff and just started taking his blood pressure. Again, not asking him to do any of these things. And so they just don't follow sort of the social rules, but they're there. They haven't lost rules. If you ask these patients, was that the appropriate thing to do, they'll say no. Oh, they know it's not appropriate. They know it's appropriate. Yeah, they say, no, I'm not supposed to.

answer your phone, but... Oh, wow. So they know better, but they can't control the impulse. Exactly. So it's not a breakdown that the rules disappear. It's that they can't apply the rules properly.

And that's true for a lot of patients, even with kids, you know, you tell them, "Don't have anything to eat before dinner because we're having dinner." And then they're sitting there having a sandwich and you say, "What did I just tell you?" You said, "Well, don't eat, but I'm hungry," right? Another sort of example is sort of the frontal lobe is not completely kind of developed.

So when I think about rules, I think about the brain. You know, the brain processes information, obviously, but it also stores information. The most important thing it does is store all sorts of information all over the brain. And I think what the frontal lobes do is they store information

rules. And what's interesting about the way it stores rules, they seem to store the rules in a hierarchical fashion. And what I mean by that is that there's different levels to rules. I like to give the example of playing golf. I tell a story a lot about my good friend Bob Knight when he hits a ball into the woods and he has to try and hit the ball out of the woods.

He's holding on to all different levels of rules on how to successfully get his ball back.

towards the green so the most simplest one is just like where you know where is the flat you know i've got to maintain the orientation to get to the flag you know so he's holding that he also had a higher level rules he knows that if he kicks the ball it's a penalty so he's not going to do that right and then another higher level rule might be if i just keep doing this you know then this is going to be healthy for me and so he's storing all this information at sort of the

at different levels of hierarchy, and he's applying it to ultimately achieve this very simple act of, or not so simple act of hitting the golf ball. So, yeah, so I just, I think about sort of the frontal cortex is able to call upon the rule in the appropriate context, and if you don't have your frontal lobes, it doesn't get

pulled up properly. And those rules must be learned, right? There's no way I can imagine that one can be born into the world with these rules sets. And I think about the two marshmallow experiment that's sort of famous now, where kids are offered

to eat one marshmallow right away or defer and get two marshmallows. These adorable videos of the kids and the various strategies they use, like turning away and poking the marshmallow. And, you know, there's some debate ongoing as to whether or not success or lack of success in deferring to the two marshmallow reward is predictive of success.

of other things in life. But leaving that aside, am I correct in assuming that that task is a frontal lobe task? The kids are given a novel rule. You can have one marshmallow now or wait patiently and then with an overcome the craving for that one marshmallow and then you'll get two. And presumably that that

experiment is engaging the frontal lobes and you know we can only speculate but um some kids are able to defer some are not um and i can imagine that at that age there's a lot of neuroplasticity um strengthening and weakening of connections in the brain on in an experience dependent way so does that mean that um

Children and perhaps adults as well can train up their prefrontal cortical abilities to strategize and defer in a way that's adaptive.

Absolutely. I mean, definitely you can learn strategies to not only sort of learn rules, but how to apply goals. When you start to think about that task in particular, some of it has to do with sort of maintaining a goal and maintaining a goal at different, you know, timescales, right? And children tend to sort of act on goals that are much more short, on a shorter timescale. You know, I'm going to have the sandwich right now because I'm hungry, as opposed to wait till lunchtime.

till dinner, which is a longer term goal. And so yeah, this default to sort of the shorter, you can learn that maintaining a longer type goal can be much more beneficial than the short term goal, even though it doesn't seem obvious. And we all learn that, right? As we...

As we get older, we keep our eye on the ball of sort of more long-term goals. And that's very predictive of how successful we can be. The farther out we can maintain a goal. And that's what the prefrontal cortex does. It maintains goals and then applies those goals. And if you don't apply them, then all of this executive function breaks down. Do you think that these...

and rules that the prefrontal cortical circuitry can learn and indeed does learn can generalize. So for instance, when I, my first year of college was a disaster for reasons that aren't interesting right now. But then when I came back my sophomore year, really spring of my freshman year, I was like, okay, it's on. I had to rescue myself. And so one of the things I used to do was I would study and I would subjugate

set a timer. So I refused to get up, even if I had to use the restroom very, very badly, I would set up all sorts of behavioral constraints. And I like to think that I was building up my prefrontal ability to refocus on the material. And fortunately for me, there were no smartphones back then. It was much easier internet. Now we had email, but no real internet browsing to speak of.

And I like to think that the – I sometimes call it – and this is terrible to call it this because it's not nearly exhaustive of the underlying function. But I call it sort of like limbic friction. It's like there's this friction that one feels mentally. Like you want to get up. You want to use the restroom. You want to eat something. You want to call a friend. But you stay focused mentally.

on the task at hand, do you think that that business of quote unquote staying focused on the task at hand can generalize because of the sensations it generates in the body? And then you go, oh, yeah, this is familiar. This is just like studying, but in a different context, one stays focused. Or do you think that the prefrontal cortex is so context specific that it needs to learn

the rules for every individual situation. And then this has all sorts of implications for behavioral restraint and focus and attention deficit. So if you could just speculate, I know a number of people are interested in how they can be more focused and people often defer to like what supplement, what drug? Okay, those are interesting conversations. But I think

ultimately we're talking about neural circuitry. Yeah, I mean, it absolutely can generalize. That's been a frustrating thing into trying to develop what we call cognitive therapy, where we teach, we try to improve someone's memory ability, or we try to improve someone's executive function ability. The disappointing early results was always that, yeah, they get very good at the tasks that you've trained them at, but it doesn't seem to generalize to anything else. So if you teach them a

you know a task they can do amazing things like match a finger to a color to a shape and put together all sorts of rules and then and they're really good at that task very quickly and then nothing's really changed in their real life but but i think we've learned on how to sort of and how to try and make it uh translate to real life and and so for example there's there's a therapy called goal management training which is developed um by

by Brian Levine and colleagues at the Rotman Research Institute of Toronto, where they've been very successful in teaching patients how to improve your executive function and how to make that translate into your real world. But it's very hard work, it's very therapist driven. It requires a series of trainings. For example, people learn... They develop individual projects like planning a meal or planning a family vacation,

planning a podcast, and then they work through what's involved in that sort of very specific project, how you stay focused, how you don't just get distracted, how you keep your eye on the ball, how you break it down to sub-goals, how you monitor what you're doing, how you don't let anxiety and procrastination get involved. But it's a very active sort of process. But when you add all that to it,

in a very disciplined way over the course of many hours, many weeks, it does translate patients and individuals to say, yeah, I'm just better at doing things. I mean, the whole goal is to do things, right? And I'm just better at it. I don't know what it is, but I'm not just better at what you taught me. I'm just better at other things. So I do have a lot of hope that these kind of therapies will generalize to the, you know, to people's real life. Throughout the

term limbic friction, again, not a technical or clinical or official term in any way, but just a way to kind of capture some of the interactions of the frontal cortex with other circuitry. I mean, there's far more involved in agitation and challenges focusing than the limbic system, but it certainly is involved.

When thinking about the frontal cortex, I often think about its connections with other areas of the brain. So maybe we could talk a little bit about those connections and in particular the connections from the frontal cortex to let's call it circuitry that controls reflexive behaviors. What is the nature of that circuitry? And can we make any general statements like does the frontal cortex really serve to provide a quieting

suppressive function on reflexes or is it more of an orchestra conductor where it's saying okay a little bit of that and a little bit of that and then what's what comes out in behavior or speech is something that looks very organized but is actually the the reflection of a lot of selective filtering

Yes, I mean the prefrontal cortex what's so fascinating about is that I would say it connects to every part of the brain, cortex and the subcortex and almost every part of the brain connects to it. So that right there tells you it's a pretty important area and it has to if it's going to be in this CEO, conductor type experience role.

And so it's in this privileged position just anatomically, so that gives us great insight to how important it is. And so it is connecting. And then, of course, we could talk about it, how it's connected to the body as well, how it controls heart rate and respirations as well. So it's not just the brain. But it's really interesting, like you said, is it really just sort of...

maintaining telling you what's relevant and what's not relevant or is it allowing you to switch? I think it does all those things. It definitely what we call sends these top-down signals. It's sending signals to the other brain about what you should be paying attention to and what you shouldn't be paying attention to. So for example,

If you, we've done studies with functional imaging where we have them look at pictures of faces and scenes, and that lights up the back of your brain. Your visual cortex has areas that are, can process faces and process scenes. And, but sometimes we have, you just want to pay attention to the faces and not the scenes. And other times we want you to pay attention to the scenes and not the faces. Well, you know, even though it's getting the same bottom up

the prefrontal cortex will show greater activity to the relevant information. It's sending a signal saying, "Pay attention to the faces, ignore the scenes," or vice versa. So it's directing all of this information that we're bombarded with to what's relevant. But at the same time, it's also allowing us to switch. If we now have to go switch to another task, it says, "Okay, this is not important now. We're going to move over to this next task."

this other, other task. So there's many different components of how it can, you know, how it can kind of control behavior, but it does all of these things in this incredible way that we still don't completely understand, but we know that's the source of all of this control is coming from the prefrontal cortex. I'd like to take a brief moment and thank one of our sponsors, and that's AG1. AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that also contains adaptogens.

I started taking AG1 way back in 2012. The reason I started taking it and the reason I still take it every day is that it ensures that I meet all of my quotas for vitamins and minerals. And it ensures that I get enough prebiotic and probiotic to support gut health.

Now, gut health is something that over the last 10 years we realized is not just important for the health of our gut, but also for our immune system and for the production of neurotransmitters and neuromodulators, things like dopamine and serotonin. In other words, gut health is critical for proper brain functioning.

Now, of course, I strive to consume healthy whole foods for the majority of my nutritional intake every single day. But there are a number of things in AG1, including specific micronutrients, that are hard to get from whole foods, or at least in sufficient quantities. So AG1 allows me to get the vitamins and minerals that I need, probiotics, prebiotics, the adaptogens, and critical micronutrients.

So anytime somebody asks me if they were to take just one supplement, what that supplement should be, I tell them AG1 because AG1 supports so many different systems within the body that are involved in mental health, physical health, and performance. To try AG1, go to drinkag1.com slash Huberman, and you'll get a year's supply of vitamin D3K2 and five free travel packs of AG1. Again, that's drinkag1.com slash Huberman.

You mentioned connections between the prefrontal cortex and the body. That's the first I've heard of that. And I'm not challenging that. To the contrary, I'm just intrigued by it. I'm aware that the hypothalamus and some of these deeper brain structures associated with more, let's call them primitive drives, temperature regulation,

hunger, et cetera, connect to the body. But what's the nature of some of the connections with the frontal lobes to the body? Yeah, I was just sort of talking in terms of our knowledge of how, you know, changing. I wonder if your podcast, you talked about how TMS to the prefrontal cortex can slow heart rate. So I meant in that sort of way. Got it. That, yeah, by influencing cortical function, obviously we can influence organs like the

Got it. So through some intermediate stations. Yes. Yeah. I mean, it's, you know, not to be hyperbolic, but I mean, it seems like the prefrontal cortex, what here we're referring to as the frontal lobes, are essentially the seat of what makes us human and what makes us functional or dysfunctional in a given context, right? I mean, I recall there's a syndrome, Kluver-Busey syndrome, which has some vague...

similarities to how you describe frontal cortex damage. But there, as I recall, humans or animals with that syndrome will

act in a way that's not appropriate to context, but more inappropriate. Like they'll try and eat a ceramic cup or draw with a piece of paper, which obviously won't work. It seems like with the frontal cortex, it knows that a pen is for writing. It just, the person might say, yeah, I know I'm not supposed to write this, but I'm just going to, or write with it, but I'm going to take your pen and write something inappropriate with it. But it's not that people forget that it's a pen.

So it seems like it's drawing on rule sets, but that something's intact. It's not like Clover Busey syndrome where animals and people can try and mate with inanimate objects. Right.

which is one of the more salient symptoms. I'll never forget that. Don't forget that from my cognitive neuroscience course, which you taught, by the way. Just throw that in there. So how should we think about this? And here I'm trying to get at kind of a broader understanding of brain function and context-specific behavior. So frontal cortex is like super sophisticated brain.

but it doesn't have all the information, right? It seems like someone without a frontal cortex probably knows that you write with a pen, you don't write with a piece of paper.

Yeah, I think it's, you know, we think about it as it's, you know, the frontal cortex allows us to take thought and move it towards action. And there's this disconnect between the knowledge and action and the separation of action from knowledge. And I guess I can reflect on my patients, you know, when I've seen a lot of patients with damage all over the brain.

All of the families of patients who have frontal lobe injury always say the same thing. They're just no longer that person. They're no longer my spouse. They're no longer my best friend. They're no longer my father. Just something, they can't put it into words, but they're not them anymore. There's something that's changed. Whereas if you talk to a patient with Broca's aphasia who has this inability to speak, they can't get any words out. Now, this is a devastating problem.

problem, they're still the same person. Their personality hasn't changed, they feel the same person, they just can't speak. The way they get around in the world is different. Or if you take a patient with prosopagnosia, which is this inability to recognize faces, of course the way they navigate around the world is difficult and it's not the same, but they're still the same person. So there's something really special about the frontal cortex that allows us to be, as you said, sort of who we are.

And that's the difficult part, like how does the frontal lobes allow us to sort of take who we are and translate that into knowledge? So we're not, I guess another word saying, just having knowledge isn't what makes us who we are, right? It's to be able to take that knowledge and present it in a way that allows us to live life based on our intentions and our goals and our desires.

so much of things like stoic philosophy and even online wellness culture are about having routines, overcoming reflex by just having recipes, scripts to follow each day. I certainly try to have my mornings be as what I call linear as possible. And I find it's much easier in the earlier part of the day to just

decide here's what I'm going to do, write out a list, do things in a certain sequence. If I don't do that, I go nonlinear as I refer to it and we'll get distracted and things of that sort. But earlier you mentioned sleep deprivation can impair frontal lobe function. It does seem that as the day progresses and certainly in the middle of the night, it just becomes much harder to control our thinking, maybe even our behavior, but and certainly our emotions. Is there a frontal lobe

regulation of emotional states as well. I know you have some recent work on this. I'd love to hear more. Yeah, I mean, as I was saying earlier, the frontal lobes is a big place and half of it is involved in these high level executive functions, but the other half of it is part of the limbic system. We call it the paralimic system that's involved in social and emotional behavior. And so there's this intimate relationship

back and forth between these two areas of the cortex. If you have just damage to these frontal... to these areas that are kind of in the overall frontal lobe, you will have

many different impairments that we would call social or emotional impairments and their executive function will be quite normal and then you'll have the opposite where patients with the lateral damage will have executive functions but they seem emotionally intact. But in real life when we have both these intact they're communicating with each other so right emotion and contacts is going to influence our executive function. We make bad decisions and

in stressful situations or situations we're not comfortable with, it's where we might make a better decision if it's a quiet, you know, kind of a quiet place.

But it is something that we can, I think you're right, you can sort of get into a routine and learn how to do things, you know, if you haven't very much planned out. But what's so unique about us is how we can be flexible and adaptable, right? When something novel comes up or there's something unexpected comes up, we can adapt to it. And that's really what the frontal cortex is really important for, not just sort of

making these plans, routines, and setting all the rules with being able, when things don't go right, how to, how to right the ship. Right. I will never ask you to demonize technology. I certainly use a smartphone from waking till sleep. Generally not in the middle of the night, if I can avoid it. And I generally avoid it, but I'm trying to take what we've discussed thus far and superimpose the

the notion of smartphones and ask, what are the rules? What are the algorithms that we're learning when we use these devices? And I'm not calling them adaptive or maladaptive. They're clearly here to stay. They've assisted in medicine. I'm sure it makes it easier for doctors to communicate on the ward and for clinic. And it's so useful, right? But contained in this small device,

There are things like, for instance, text messaging where unlike 20 years ago, we can have four or five different conversations very quickly while boarding a flight. There's a task switching element that was just not present in our life prior to that. Social media in particular, this notion of being able to scroll. So if we really step back from this, move one's thumb and access something.

hundreds, if not thousands of video content from distinct, which each of which has a distinct context. And so I have to imagine that kids and adults have frontal courtesies that are learning these rules. And the rule is move your thumbs, stay engaged,

either positive valence emotions or negative emotions. I mean, it's a fairly limited landscape there when you really think about it. But the algorithm that's learned is to me doesn't seem exportable. It doesn't help me prepare for a podcast at all. I know that for sure. It doesn't help me go for a run. It doesn't help me listen with more focused attention to a family member or a friend or a significant other.

It may make me more empathic or more angry. You know, we can speculate, but again, with no intention of demonizing social media, does it seem that the algorithms that are being run in our brain, I mean, are they neutral? Are they positive? Are they negative? Should we be worried?

It doesn't seem like they translate to much else. I can't see a way in which they help us be better people in other domains. Whereas reading a book line by line and then going back, oh, I didn't even remember anything from that page, going back line by line, playing a game of squash or something like that, I can see the real value of the rule sets that generalize.

Yeah, I mean, I can, you know, just historically, I grew up in a world when there was no smartphones as a resident. And so one of the most difficult things I do in practice is have to take care of patients in the emergency room. And there's a real emergency. Someone's having gun control seizures or they're having a stroke. And, you know, doing this back in the 80s or 90s and early 2000s, when you went down there and you didn't have any smartphone, you could only rely on what's in your head. And I could say now having the smartphone, it hasn't,

It doesn't help me at all, it does not help me at all in making the kind of decisions that I have to make in the emergency room. I'm trying to decide what's the problem here, what's the differential diagnosis, how should I treat it, I'm just trying to make very... going through an algorithm like you said in a common sense way and there's nothing on my phone that I can turn to to help me do that. It has helped with giving me knowledge.

Back in the day, I had to remember what the Dilantin dose was and have that in my head or go look for the piece of paper in my pocket. And so I can quickly pull up, you know, I guess I'm a little bit...

There's information that I can access that I don't have to worry about keeping every single dose in my head or keeping everything in my head, just facts in my head. But outside of that, there's nothing I can turn to that it's making me make better decisions. So I don't even need my cell phone. I don't go searching for my cell phone if I'm going to go to a room or I'm going to take a phone call. So I don't see how it's helping me.

sort of make your frontal lobe, it can't be your frontal lobes, I mean it's another way of saying it. But on the flip side, can it help you optimize frontal lobe function technology? Certainly it can. We can maybe talk about it later. There are certainly, that's one way to get learned strategies is through a device that's easily accessible and, you know, to you as opposed to a book or having a therapist in your house.

Yeah. I suppose I worry that too much of my time and other people's time, and especially young people's time is engaging in a algorithm that does not generalize for adaptive behavior elsewhere. And by comparison, you know, like a game of soccer with friends or something, right? It's social, social media is social. It's physical. Social media is not physical, but we'll rule that portion out. But there's a rule set that

There's goal-directed behavior. Presumably some of the things that happen in a game of soccer with friends translate to some other domain of life because it's a single context game of soccer. Whereas with social media, I don't know anybody that goes and looks at one account.

and that's it, and absorbs the information, maybe comments, has an interaction, and goes. It's hundreds or thousands of contexts. So is there any risk or perhaps benefit to being able to get this very detailed portal into so many contexts per unit time? I mean, the forebrain's never had done that in the course of human history, as far as I know. Yeah, I mean, I think there is a risk, but what pops to mind, you know, having...

is watching them navigate in their cars to places totally dependent on Google Maps. I think you're probably old enough to remember real maps. I still have one in my car. I love paper maps. I love maps. Right, where you had to really figure out, you know, you had to go to a certain place and you had to either look at the map or...

or ask, stop at a gas station and ask, these skills were something that you learned and you developed and it was problem solving. And that's all gone now. I mean, it's, I wonder even if sometimes if,

if people even know the direction they're going, whether it's west, north, or what town they're in, because they're just following the directions. So we'll see. I just can't imagine that that learned skill is not going to be detrimental to us at some point and generalize in a bad way, right, as opposed to...

a good way. So I don't, yeah, it does definitely worry me. But like you said, there's nothing on the phone that helps you plan a podcast, nothing that helps me in the emergency room, nothing helps a professor when he's giving a lecture. So I agree with you that the sort of having your head buried in a cell phone, I'm not, yeah, I don't see it being healthy for your frontal lobes. Let's talk about working memory.

some years back, but still now you use working memory tasks and experiments in your laboratory. If you would be so kind as to explain what working memory is. And then I'd love to talk about some of the work you've done exploring the role of dopamine in working memory, because this is so critical to everyday life. And I know dopamine is a bit of a buzzword these days, but the listeners of this podcast anyway are

are pretty sophisticated in terms of knowing that dopamine is not just about reward. It's about motivation and goal-directed behavior. And I think dopamine intrigues for a good reason, that it does govern a lot of our quality of life. So what's working memory?

Yeah, I mean, working memory, it's interesting. I started studying it about 30 years ago, and I don't think I realized how important it was when I started. But what we mean by working memory is this ability to hold information in mind when it's no longer accessible to us. So if you tell me your telephone number and I have to

put it into my phone, you know, it's no longer there, you just told me, but I'll hold it in my working memory until I can punch it into my phone. It doesn't have to be something that comes from the outside world. I could hold up, you know, I can pull up my own, if I'm filling out a form and I want to pull up my social security number, I can hold that in mind too until I put it down. So,

When you think about it, it's a very important ability that we have that we do very flawlessly. And what I've learned more about working memory is the working part of it. It's not just this passive holding information in mind, but it's being able to do things.

things with the information. It's being able to, you know, when we do a math problem, which we don't do that much now that we have calculators, but if you do that in your head, you're able to sort of manipulate the information and do the different parts of the problem. Or even if you're trying to find someone in a crowd and you're holding on to some face, you're able to hold that face in mind and cross-check it and search. And so there's operations to working memory. It's not just this passive thing.

maintenance. So when we started to think about working memory in that way, we started to realize how important it is for it's, you know, I think of it as the foundation for cognition. Just think about reading comprehension. You can't understand this conversation if you can't hold in mind what's going on, you know, earlier in the conversation or when you're reading a book, you know, remembering the sentence before it. So it just predicts all these abilities that allows us to read, to

to plan, to organize, and all the sort of executive functions that we're doing. We have to hold in mind rules, we have to hold in mind goals, we have to hold in mind all of these things in order to just carry out behavior.

So it's really come a long way in terms of how people are thinking about it. I know that Matt Walker said that sleep is our superpower, but I guess one way to sort of use his term, while we're awake, working memory is really our superpower because it allows us to translate, as we said, sort of our knowledge into action by holding this information in mind as we're thinking about what we want to do.

if we're going to think about dopamine in the context of working memory, is dopamine an accelerator on working memory? Is it a facilitator? I mean, what is dopamine doing for working memory? And maybe we could talk a little bit about the circuitry. Um,

I've talked about dopamine before on this podcast, but there's a good chance that some of the people listening to this haven't heard those episodes. So maybe we could just quickly review the three major circuits for dopamine and the one that's relevant for working memory. Yeah. Let me start with the working memory, the circuitry for working memory, because one of the...

Important things about working memory is the other type of memory is long-term memory. Working memory is short-lived. It's only as long as you're able to rehearse it, and then it disappears. Whereas what we call long-term memory, remembering what you had for breakfast or your vacation, this is information that gets consolidated and gets put into a

a more durable form that we call long-term memory. And the interesting thing about memory is that these are separate systems. Everything from working memory just doesn't pass into long-term memory. They're two completely different systems

in two completely different parts of the brain that seem to control it. So working memory, the frontal cortex seems to be very important for working memory. When we are holding information in line, the neurons, the brain cells in the frontal lobes are active and they stay kind of active as long as we're holding on that information.

And they're more active when the information is relevant. And if we... we get distracted, they'll get less active. So kind of the frontal lobes kind of track your

you know, track the memory that you're holding in mind. Another important thing about the circuitry is that if we're holding in mind, say, digits, you know, the phone number, well, that information is in your back of the brain. And so the frontal lobes is sort of keeping information in the back of the brain active because it's connected to the visual areas. It's able to sort of keep that information active. And so what we've learned is that there's not these buffers in the brain

where if you're holding verbal information it's in this little buffer and if you're holding visual information it's in another buffer. The whole brain acts as a buffer and the frontal lobe can call up any part of the brain and keep that part of the brain active as it's trying to hold this information in line. So the mechanism for working memory is just this persistent neural activity within the frontal lobes. And so then the question is what does dopamine do?

Well dopamine is one of the neuromodulators that are made in the brainstem and it projects up to different parts of the brain. There's a system that goes up into what we call the basal ganglia which is important for motor function and there's another dopaminergic system that goes up to the frontal lobes. And what was discovered was that if you deplete dopamine, working memory

drops. You get a significant impairment of working memory if you deplete dopamine, and if you replace it, then your working memory will be improved. And so dopamine seems to be a modulator to help this persistent activity stay persistent during the time that you need to keep this information in mind. Am I

reaching too far to draw an analogy between dopamine's role in working memory, that is to keep information online, and the other established role of dopamine, which is for movement, for the generation of smooth movement, as evidenced by conditions like Parkinson's, where people lack dopaminergic neurons or have damage to dopaminergic neurons and have challenges in generating smooth movement. What I'm essentially asking is,

Can we think of dopamine as facilitating physical movement through one circuit, but also kind of mental movement?

thought movement, I'm thinking about for those just listening and not watching, I'm kind of rubbing my index and middle finger against my thumb, just keeping something online. It's sort of a movement of thought or information, and then you kind of chuck it away and bring out the next information. Is that appropriate? Yeah, I think that's a good way of thinking about it. And one might wonder, well, how can dopamine be important for memory, but also be important for movement? And it's really simple. It's just that it's acting on different circuits. The

The neurons that go to the motor areas that carry dopamine, when dopamine is expressed there and boosted there, then it will be involved in movement. And lack of dopamine in the basal ganglion will lead to neurological disorders like Parkinson's disease that has severe movement issues.

difficulty. But when it's acting in the frontal cortex and expressing the frontal cortex, then it's going to improve working memory. So it's just the nature of where the circuits are, where the dopamine is, that's allowing it to have different kinds of actions. And that's for all transmitters. The reason why acetylcholine seems to be more important for long-term memory is because it

it's projecting to the hippocampus, which we know is another area that's important for memory. And that's why acylcholine doesn't boost your working memory, but dopamine does and vice versa. I'd like to take a quick break to acknowledge our sponsor, Element.

Elements is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing you don't. That means zero sugar and the appropriate ratios of the electrolytes, sodium, magnesium, and potassium. And that correct ratio of electrolytes is extremely important because every cell in your body, but especially your nerve cells, your neurons, relies on electrolytes in order to function properly.

So when you're well hydrated and you have the appropriate amount of electrolytes in your system, your mental functioning and your physical functioning is improved. I drink one packet of Element dissolved in about 16 to 32 ounces of water when I wake up in the morning, as well as while I exercise. And if I've sweat a lot during that exercise, I often will drink a third Element packet dissolved in about 32 ounces of water after I exercise.

Element comes in a variety of different flavors, all of which I find really tasty. I like the citrus, I like the watermelon, I like the raspberry. Frankly, I can't pick just one. It also comes in chocolate and chocolate mint, which I find tastes best if they are put into water, dissolved, and then heated up. I tend to do that in the winter months because, of course, you don't just need hydration on hot days and in the summer and spring months, but also in the winter when the temperatures are cold and the environment tends to be dry.

If you'd like to try Element, you can go to drinkelement, spelled L-M-N-T dot com slash Huberman, to try a free sample pack. Again, that's drinkelement.com slash Huberman. So drilling a little bit more deeply into the role of dopamine and working memory, you did some really lovely experiments showing that if

People who have low levels of dopamine increase their dopamine pharmacologically. I think the drug that was used was bromocriptine that working memory improves. Conversely, if one depletes dopamine pharmacologically, working memory gets worse. But as I recall, there was an important baseline that is important because it really mattered in terms of the outcome. Meaning if somebody already had relatively high levels of dopamine in this circuit, they

increasing dopamine further with bromocriptine didn't impart a benefit and might have even made their working memory worse. So there's a kind of inverted U-shaped function to this. How does one know whether or not their baseline dopamine is low, medium, or high?

Ergo, how do they know whether or not they would want to explore going about increasing dopamine through any number of different approaches? Right. Well, most people probably have optimal dopamine, but there's a significant percentage that probably have too little or maybe too much. And unfortunately, we can't measure it in the blood. There isn't a blood test that I'm aware of that can measure dopamine.

Dopamine because it's stuck in the brain. Peripheral dopamine in the blood is not a good readout. It's not a good readout, yeah. And especially when you're talking about dopamine in areas like prefrontal cortex. And so we don't have a good readout there. There's invasive procedures like positron emission tomography where we can inject a radioisotope.

that tags dopamine and then we can measure how much, we can do a scan that actually shows us how much dopamine. This scan was originally developed to show Parkinson's disease that you can diagnose Parkinson's disease by showing that there's less dopamine in patients that have Parkinson's disease by looking at this scan. Obviously it's invasive, you're injecting a real isotope, it's expensive and it's not something

we could all do. But we had used it to show that it correlates very strongly with your working memory capacity. So how much information you can hold online, if you can hold four or five or six letters when I do a span task, correlated with how much dopamine we can see in the PET scan. So that would be a way that we could do it. So if you were to read out a string of a few numbers or letters, and I can remember all of those a few moments later,

perhaps my baseline dopamine levels are moderate in the normal range. Whereas if I couldn't keep that online, that might be reflective of lower baseline dopamine levels. Is that right? Yeah, it's a very strong proxy for dopamine. So if your working memory capacity is seven letters or numbers, when I say 4371506, if you get that. 4371506.

Get them all back to you very quickly. You probably have more baseline dopamine than someone who has five. So it's a proxy for measuring someone's dopamine. So that's one way to do it. And that's actually how we did it in our original studies. We actually grouped...

individuals based on whether their capacity based on this behavioral measure was high or low. And like you said those who work that can only hold five or six letters if we gave them bromocriptine which was the dopaminergic agonist we improved their working memory.

We got them into sort of an optimal level. But those who were already high, we actually made them, we got them worse. And the moral of that story was that more is just not better. We're trying to get people optimal. And so the real question is, you know, if we want to get people optimal like you were inferring, you have to know what their dopamine is. Where are you on this inverted U?

Another way of doing it is through genetic studies. So we don't mean all neurotransmitters have to be broken down and reuptake into the brain cell in order to be used again, and there's different ways of doing it. In some cells it gets transported back into the brain cell, and other places there's an enzyme that breaks it down. Well, there's an enzyme called COMT,

that breaks down dope being in the prefrontal cortex specifically. In a large percentage of individuals that enzyme is either overactive or underactive. Probably about 25% of individuals it's overactive and another 25% it's underactive. So probably half the population. Now this is going to vary, depend on other...

where you live and where you come from and things, but maybe half the population either has an underactive enzyme or overactive enzyme. If you have an underactive enzyme then actually more dopamine sits around and you actually have more dopamine than others and if you have an overactive enzyme it's the opposite. So we've actually shown that if you now go and genotype people with a simple saliva test and figure out do they have this genetic what we call polymorphism where just one amino acid gets changed

and the enzyme becomes either active or underactive, we can do the same thing as grouping them by their capacity. Those that have the low dopamine, we will make them better, and those who have sort of baseline high dopamine will make them worse. Super interesting. Maybe we could talk about bromocriptine a little bit, and I'm not encouraging people to run out and take bromocriptine. Bromocriptine, as you mentioned, is a dopamine agonist,

relatively short acting? Yeah, four or five hours, six hours. So it kicks in about 90 minutes after, as I recall you saying, I've never taken it. How do people feel when they're on bromocriptine? I mean, when I hear dopamine agonist, I mean, there are a lot of illicit drugs like cocaine, methamphetamine that are increased dopamine, but then again, chocolate.

sex and food increased dopamine, but the kinetics, the time course and the levels are different for each of those things. Dopamine, of course, being a currency of motivation and reward, not directly related to any one compound. But I would think that based on the data you just described that, and given the fact that there are a number of people out there with challenges in working memory, attention, task switching, et cetera, that

there would be a strong interest on the part of the pharmaceutical companies at least, and certainly the general public in things like promocryptine to increase dopamine, to increase working memory, given it is our superpower.

Yeah, I mean, one of the most disappointing things to me in my career has been that pharmaceutical companies have not picked up on this idea that we could improve cognition and very specifically improve chiropractic with very specific neuromodulators. The discovery that depletion of dopamine and not other transmitters impairs working memory was made in 1979. When I heard...

Pat Gomer used to talk about this as a resident, I was just amazed that there could be a single transmitter can change a single behavior. I was seeing very complicated behavioral deficits and it just seemed impossible to me that there could be such a tight link between a single neuromodulator and a single cognitive process and just opened the door for me that this really could be an incredibly beneficial therapy for anyone with executive function or frontal lobe function.

But unfortunately, there's never been a pharmaceutical company that's tried to develop a drug for improving cognition to this day. That's crazy. I mean, it's crazy for several reasons. One is that the data are clearly there. Two, these drugs are already established. It's not like they have to go through safety trials. Again, that's already been done. But mostly because regardless of whether one is a fan of the pharmaceutical industry or hates it,

The pharmaceutical industry in principle can make a ton of money doing this, so I would think that they'd be heavily incentivized to do it. So why have they turned a blind eye on this? I'm not sure. I mean, when I realized that I could test these drugs in healthy individuals, that if I gave them in low enough doses, they were safe, and I had so much experience of them in patients that I felt comfortable doing it,

Then I started asking pharmaceutical companies, you know, do you want to get involved here? This should be done, I can't do this by myself, we need to have real trials and real studies of how this will help, you know. And just it was, you know, their eyes would always cross and never got any sort of traction. It always went back to sort of disease, you know, what disease are you...

curing, you know, what's the market for it? Is it a Parkinson's disease thing? Is it an Alzheimer's disease thing? And this has been a general problem with neurology, it's very disease centric. It's always focused on how can we develop a treatment for Alzheimer's or traumatic brain injury or stroke as opposed to how can we develop a treatment for working memory dysfunction which is a problem across

So the answer to your earlier question is these drugs are very safe. We give them in such low doses to healthy individuals, they don't even know, they can't even tell the difference between the placebo and the drug. Really? They don't even know which one they're on. So they're not buzzing, thinking, oh, this feels good, and my working memory is better.

They have no idea. They don't even know their working memory is better until we show them that their working memory is better. Love it. Yeah. So they're truly blind to what's going on. Bromocriptine is but one of the dopamine agonists. You can think of a few others, cabergoline, like other things like that. Do any of these dopamine agonists...

exert this impact on working memory? Or does it vary by drug because different dopamine agonists sort of hit different receptor pathways and things like that? - Yeah, no, it's not specifically the drug. I mean, the reason for bromocriptine is that it's the oldest and it's the one I was most comfortable with. I had to be comfortable with it clinically before I'd give it to undergraduates at Penn or Berkeley, so there's nothing special. But other agonists work similarly.

There's a drug that's developed, which is a COMT inhibitor, which actually inhibits this enzyme that we're talking about, and that also will improve, will have the same function. There's been some work that norepinephrine,

also seems to be helpful with working memory. It's maybe not as potent as the dopaminergic. And that's the point I want to make, another disappointing thing about this whole field of the pharmacology of cognition. You know, I wrote a paper as a resident. You know, sometimes you're tending to say, hey, can you write this review paper for us? And I wrote one as a resident called The Pharmacology of Cognition where I looked at all the animal literature on animals

giving neuromodulators acetylcholine, dopamine or whatever. And there was a lot of animal literature supporting that this would work in humans, but what was more striking to me was that it wasn't always just a single neurotransmitter. There were studies where you'd give dopamine and it wouldn't do anything, you give acetylcholine and it wouldn't do anything, but if you gave a low dose of both,

it would be really effective. So these trans-transmissive systems don't act in isolation, so we need to also study sort of how the combinations work. And that's where the pharmaceutical companies have the infrastructure to do these kind of studies. It's very hard to do in a single lab to do multiple drugs at one time and then try and determine all the different interactions. Maybe we could talk about a couple of other drugs that are –

and have FDA approval are known to be safe in the right context that it seems would fit the bill here for improving working memory. One is Welbutrin. I can never pronounce that. As far as I know, it's a

epinephrine or norepinephrine agonist. You just mentioned that increasing epinephrine may have a positive impact on working memory and to some extent a dopamine agonist. Is there any evidence that

um, wellbutrin can improve working memory. Yeah. And anything that, um, boost norepinephrine can do it. The one that we've used that's most used is guanfacine, which is actually a blood pressure medication. So that's starting to gain some traction. In fact, I think there was a study with COVID with brain fog for COVID showing that improved symptoms with it. So there's actually some trials now that are looking at guanfacine. And so I would say anything that boosts norepinephrine would be, would be helpful. Um,

But then again, I don't want to leave out the other transmitters. Serotonin, you know, increasing serotonin, increasing acetylcholine boosts other cognitive processes and then...

you know, in a way they can help working memory. We talked about working memory being this foundation. Well, if you give acetylcholine and it kind of boosts memory, well, that can indirectly help your executive function. Or if you give a drug that improves your focus, then that can indirectly help, you know, working memory. So what I'm really pushing for is not just a single, you know, it's going to be one drug, you know, one drug. It's going to be a cocktail. And we have to not only figure out what the

cocktail is, but also figure out who we're giving it to. Link it to the person's own makeup of their own neurochemistry. When we get to a point where we'll know, we can map out sort of everyone's dopamine, norepinephrine, serotonin levels, and then we'll make real progress in helping them. Because right now, I sort of say...

With my students, what we're doing is just like cutting open the skull and just sort of pouring it onto the brain. We're not actually doing that. We're not actually doing it, but it seems that way. The precision is not there yet. Well, it's great that you developed this cognitive task that can be a proxy for dopamine levels. The cognitive task, again, being...

how many number or letter strings somebody can remember, basically working memory performance. There are a lot of tests out there that claim they can assess dopamine and serotonin acetylcholine levels from a blood draw. I've heard of the Dutch test, I've never taken it. But a few minutes ago, you said that really one needs to do positron emission tomography imaging, which is fairly labor intensive. Most people don't have access to one of those.

It's a clinical tool. So there are behavioral proxies, there's neuroimaging, but also to my knowledge, I don't know that there's any blood draw that will say, hey, your serotonin levels are low or your dopamine levels are moderate, et cetera. There are a lot of companies that market these, but are you aware of any clinical or other tools for getting an accurate read of neurotransmitter levels in a person's brain aside from neuroimaging?

No, and it's even more complicated than it seems because the dopaminergic system is complicated because it's not only just the prefrontal cortex, as we talked about, it's also the basal ganglia. And so not only do we have to measure dopamine just generally levels, we have to measure the balance of the dopamine in this triatom and the prefrontal cortex. There's a model of dopamine function and its relation to exoskeleton.

executive function that has to do with sort of the balance between these two systems that don't mean in the prefrontal cortex is promoting sort of stability, it's keeping information in mind, it's keeping these representations stable.

Whereas the dopamine in the basal ganglia, what it's doing is allowing you to update and refresh, you know, the information that you're holding in mind, this sort of stability versus flexibility. So if you have too much dopamine in the frontal cortex, it can lead to a very rigid state where you don't let anything in. And if you have too much dopamine in the striatum and you get too flexible, then you can get very distractible. So

there's a sort of balance of dopamine. So it's not just how much dopamine you have in your brain, it's how much, what's the balance of the dopamine. So I don't see a blood test as ever giving us

that information, but I do see there being a brain test that can give us this kind of information of the two or at least a proxy for it. So what I was thinking about when you were talking about asking this question, you know, for example, if you measure pupillary, pupil dilation, that's a pretty good proxy for the neurodegenerative system. Right, so at a given, people will wonder how to do it. We're not going to...

go into too much detail here, but at a given brightness in the room, what we call luminance, the pupil tends to be smaller when it's bright and larger when you're in a dim room. That's sort of obvious. But at a given luminance, the more alert, aroused somebody is, arousal as a general term here, not talking about a particular kind of arousal, then the pupil tends to be more dilated. It gets bigger.

the more norepinephrine is in the system. So if somebody's pupils are really big in bright light,

that person's got a lot of epinephrine, adrenaline in their system. Do you use this clinically? Like when someone comes in and they have those big old pupils, and you're like, okay, they're probably on a stimulant. Yeah, I mean, a lot of what neurology does is try to look for these windows into the brain. And so I think there are a number of windows into the brain that we're going to be able to develop that can reflect these neuromodulatory systems. So that's why I've been so interested in developing biomarkers, because really what a neurobiomarker is, is trying to

develop something you can measure easily and simply and cheaply with, you know, but gives you information about how the brain is working. And so that's a neuroprinephrine biomarker. We're comparing capacities that don't mean biomarker and we're getting better at that. But again, we're not putting enough emphasis on it, in my opinion, to really sort of help, you know, improve brain health. Have you ever tried bromocriptine?

Very early on, but it's such a low, you know, at the dose that my subjects were getting. But like I said, it's so low you don't feel anything. And I should say, even with patients that take it, they rarely get any side effects. Sometimes with these drugs, because there's peripheral dopamine, they can get nausea or vomiting. But it's extremely well tolerated. You don't get any.

any anything feeling from it does it change reaction time it does and that's always the question of of how much of this is that we're just sort of speeding up we're just sort of making them faster but for all the work we've done it's pretty convincing that it's not just how fast you're doing it you're doing it better uh you might find this entertaining um some years ago I learned that athletes

We're taking bromocriptine pre-Olympics and in the Olympics. I think it's a banned substance now. And the athletes that were taking it, don't ask me how I know this, but I could tell you offline. And I'm not one of these athletes, nor was I supplying the bromocriptine.

We're using it because they were sprinters. And it turns out that a lot of the sprint races are won by being first out the blocks. There are other factors as well. But that reaction time, you know, hundreds of milliseconds are the difference between podium and no podium.

And bromocriptine was one of the drugs used. It was not on the banned substance list. Just a reminder that every Olympics you see, there are lots of things being used that are not on the banned substance list. And I'm not trying to be disparaging. I think there's just a lot of interest in augmenting neuromodulation for nervous system function. Bromocriptine was top of the list at that time. I think it's on the banned list now. There's a lot of

use of pharmacology now on college campuses and in high school and even in elementary schools and sometimes by parents for their kids to try and improve cognitive function. Most typically the use of Adderall, Vyvanse, Ritalin and other stimulants which are noradrenergic dopaminergic agonists. Okay, so...

With the disclaimer, caveat, whatever you want to call it, that those decisions should always be made with a trained psychiatrist monitoring things. What are your thoughts about pharmacology for enhancing cognitive function, given that the landscape of society is challenging and people want to perform well, they need to be able to focus? We've got smartphones distracting us. And to some extent,

You know, one could say, oh, well, it's cheating to use pharmacology, but a cup of coffee is a bit of a noradrenergic agonist. Absolutely. And certainly improves my focus as long as I don't drink too much of it. Right. Yeah, what are your thoughts? Yeah, I think it, you know, it kind of gets back to...

We talked about there being an optimal, you know, optimal level of dopamine in your brain. I think if you think about it as just more and more and more is better and that more is better, then there's really no end. There's really no end. How do you know how much you should be taking? That experiment was run in the 80s. It's called the cocaine culture of Wall Street.

in the 80s. There were movies about it and it doesn't lead to good places. Right, right. So I'm all for optimizing function. I want to optimize brain health. And if you have an underactive enzyme that makes your dopamine levels, then I'm all for trying to optimize that along with everything else we need to optimize in the brain. So if we could figure out who is sort of on the lower end and boost them up, I'm all for that. The problem is we don't know if they're on the high end

And some of these athletes were actually making themselves worse. We know for sure. I mean, these are healthy Penn and Berkeley undergraduates that we made them worse on working memory tests, you know, by increasing their dopamine. Just a little amount, just tipped them over just a little amount.

And so without the knowing, then it seems like it's not well informed. The other thing is if we're going to do this, we should do it right. I think drugs like Adderall and Ritalin, they were developed...

because they helped patients, but they weren't necessarily developed with knowing how exactly they worked. I mean, that's how the pharmaceutical company works sometimes. SSRIs too. Yeah. I mean, it works, so let's do it. I'm all for that as a physician. But if I had my choice, you know, drugs that boost up multiple cysts, all the catecholamines, the ones that boost up dopamine, epinephrine, and norminephrine, I would steer away from those because you have no control over

over how you're modulating the system. Again, I was sort of talking about a cocktail. It may be a little bit of dopamine and a little more norepinephrine, but if you take something like Ritalin Adderall, you just get the same amount. So it's kind of, if I was to start to sort of experiment, then I wouldn't use Adderall or Ritalin as the drug that I think would help, even though they're clinically sort of useful. I use things like bromocriptine and guanfacine where they...

can modulate a very specific drug. And then, and then, yeah, then the goal is to, to optimize. And that's what we're trying to do with cognitive therapy and everything's sleeping better and better nutrition. All of these are aiming to optimize, not, you know, reach some super human potential. Right. Just bring out the best in, in people's abilities. Right. And I'm, I'm so glad you mentioned sleep. I would say, you know, sleep is the,

bedrock. It's the foundation of mental health, physical health, and performance. I mean, without that, pharmacology might bridge you for an afternoon, but you're going to pay the piper somehow. Friend and colleague Matt Walker obviously has been beating that drum for a while. What about drugs like modafinil, which are thought to be true cognitive enhancers as opposed to drugs that just kind of

designed to ramp up levels of alertness, as many of the drugs we're discussing do. Yeah, it's hard to know. I mean, I think certain drugs just improve general abilities. Either they speed how fast you can process it or how efficient you can process or narrow the focus of your attention, and that just helps all abilities. So it's hard to say. I think there just has to be more work on really understanding what specifically works

you know, these drugs are doing. That's why broncryptine, the dopaminerg story has been so interesting because it's a very specific effect with a very specific mechanism. I like to see that be done with other neuromodulators. Maybe we could talk a bit about some of the disease conditions that you treat and the role of working memory and dopamine in those conditions.

as well as other transmitter systems. You know, one subject that we haven't talked about on this podcast previously, but is of tremendous interest to people is traumatic brain injury or concussion, even mild concussion. And before we were recording today, we were talking about football, but just want to remind people that football is just one instance of

an opportunity to get a concussion or traumatic brain injury. Most traumatic brain injury and concussion is not due to football. It just gets a lot of the attention, but you've got bicycle accidents, car accidents, playground accidents. Maybe you could list off a few more, but how common is TBI in concussion? And maybe you could just

perhaps list out some of the other situations where you see a lot of this, that it's a bit more cryptic that people wouldn't necessarily think that sport or that population gets TBI, but they do. Yeah, I think concussion is much more prevalent than we realize. And the numbers have gone up and up, not because it's becoming more common, it's just it's becoming more recognized. And I think, you know, we underestimated and trivialized sort of what a concussion is, you know, that it's just a

Something that is, you know, just you're going to recover from it. I mean, still the old school thinking by a lot of neurologists is that everyone gets better within a couple of months. You know, just wait it out and you'll get better. That's just the normal thing.

time course of concussion. But as we've studied it more, we realized that there's actually quite a large percentage of people who a year out, they're still suffering problems. They still feel like they're not mentally clear and they still are sensitive to light and they still feel a little dizzy and just a symptom, you know, there's a host of symptoms that just one year later after a concussion where they didn't even lose consciousness, you know, that's something that they may not

have even talked to their doctor about is lingering.

And so it's a real, we call this persistent post-concussion syndrome and that's the most worrisome to me because it is true that most concussions will recover. Luckily the brain is incredibly resilient, incredibly plastic and it will heal itself. But there are a lot of patients where it just persists and those are the most worrisome to me because we don't have very good interventions to try and help that and I don't think we take these patients very seriously when they're complaining of something that seems very

vague and not very specific to most doctors. What do you tell a patient who comes in and has clearly had a concussion, mild or severe concussion? Maybe a car accident, maybe a sports injury. Maybe they were knocked out cold, maybe not. But they're having some headaches, some photophobia, sensitivity, just feeling not right. I've had a couple of these, unfortunately. And you just feel off. You don't feel quite right.

And some of that manifests as focus issues. This was some years ago. I like to think I'm through it. I've had scans and I'm good. So thank goodness. But what do you tell them besides don't get another one?

Yeah, well, first of all, I explain what a concussion is. What I found in neurology, a lot of what patients want to know is just they just want to understand their problem. They're not walking in expecting a cure. Just understanding what it is, having someone understand what happened to them is very helpful and comforting. So,

What we mean by concussion and in the clinical world we use mild traumatic brain injury kind of synonymous with concussion. It basically is a tearing of axons. The brain cells have these long fibers that communicate with each other and they're called axons. And when the brain violently moves forward and backwards, if you're in a car accident and you have your seatbelt on and you suddenly hit, you go from 50 to 0,

your head violently goes forward and violently goes backwards and that angular force actually tears and stretches axons in the brain so if you've had a concussion you have torn some axons i mean luckily we have billions of them and so if you tear a couple of thousand you you will recu you will recover but you you have torn axon that's a real neurologic it's a real brain

injury, even if you haven't lost consciousness and you've only had symptoms for a couple of days. But there's a correlation, the longer you've lost consciousness and the longer your symptoms last, the more axons you've torn, this kind of a direct relationship between the two. So the mechanism is these torn axons, so now neurosurgeons don't communicate with each other and you know that the brain, different brain regions are not communicating with each other.

And it turns out the most common place for axon satyr is in the frontal lobes. And so now we talked about all these things that the frontal lobes do to orchestrate the rest of the brain. Well, it has some injured pathways. And that's why a lot of the symptoms that patients have are these kind of mild executive symptoms. This mental fogginess that they're describing is just this inability to

to get things done. They don't lose knowledge of who they are. They don't forget their name or forget where they live or lose memories from the past or anything like that, but they don't officially get things done as well as they used to. It only takes a little bit of a drop, right? People think you have to have a big drop in performance to have a real-life impact. Just a 1% drop and you're having a hard time doing your podcast or teaching a lecture or whatever you might do. A 1% drop.

sounds like a, um, a frighteningly small change, um, required to, to negatively impact life. So, um, how about a poor night's sleep? I mean, what kind of drop in prefrontal cortical function are we looking at, um, from a, let's say I normally get seven or eight hours or six to eight hours and I suddenly only get three or four. Are we talking a significant detriment? I,

I do think so. I do think that, yeah, that it is significant, a poor night's sleep. And we all notice that. I mean, it's very obvious. I mean, and, you know, it's hard to sort of quantify. I'm a baseball fan, so I can quantify it like if you think about it in a pitcher and how fast they throw, you know,

a small drop for them, someone who's throwing 100 miles an hour, just a small drop turns them from really elite to someone mediocre. Maybe it's more of a 10% drop, but a still relatively small drop can have a huge impact. I think people think that just because you're a little bit off, that's not a big deal. You kind of work through it. And that's what most doctors say. Just plow through it. Just work your way through it. You're going to get better. As opposed to saying...

yeah, you really had a brain injury. This is what happened. We need to rehabilitate you just like we would do if you tore your anterior cruciate ligament. I don't know why tearing your cruciate ligament or your Achilles tendon gets more interest than tearing axons in your brain. It's amazing to me that there's more emphasis on orthopedic injuries than brain injuries. Yeah, I don't know why that is either. I think the brain is mysterious enough that...

most people and many clinicians just kind of back away with hands raised. But if you are in the field of neurology or psychiatry, I suppose that then one has officially signed on to try and resolve these matters. So for somebody that has a traumatic brain injury or low level concussion, excuse me, would part of the primary advice be to try and get one's sleep as good as possible?

Given that sleep deprivation can compound traumatic brain injury, induce deficits in working memory, and who knows, maybe a good portion of the deficits in working memory due to traumatic brain injury and concussion is because of the sleep deprivation that it can cause. So it can get circular.

Not only that, but one of the most common symptoms that my patients with acution have is their sleep is disruptive. And that's true in neurology. It's fascinating. Almost every neurological disorder, my patients complain of their sleep. And I started asking, you know, not a lot of neurologists ask you how your sleep is.

You know, but I remember back from my residency one of the first things my attending would do when we got to the ward is, "I had to sleep last night." And it's just across the board. You know, patients are not falling asleep, they're not staying asleep and it's... we still don't understand why just brain injury does that. So almost every concussion patient says, "I'm not sleeping well," which then compounds, you know, compounds the problem. So optimizing sleep obviously, optimizing nutrition.

There's a question about activity. It used to be that we used to recommend, you know, you had a concussion, you should don't go to work, you know, it's...

Just take it easy for a while, don't exercise. Keep the blinds drawn. But now it's the idea that you should really get up and moving. You got to do what you can tolerate. You don't want to give yourself more of a headache or more light sensitivity. But as much as you can tolerate is the thought these days about sort of promoting recovery. And then really getting your brain...

back working. I think, you know, a lot of my patients they're off from work for a couple weeks and they feel fine and they think they're pretty much normal and then the first day of work is a complete disaster because until you actually test it in real life you don't know how what kind of troubles you have. So I don't recommend going back full steam but I do recommend going back trying to build up these skills again. And then I think we need to develop therapies that people will use.

Things like goal management training which involves a therapist and health insurance doesn't pay for this. So 99% of my patients don't get any help by any kind of intervention, unfortunately. But now we talk about technology, things like Brain HQ. Do you know about Brain HQ? So Mike Merzenich, which I know you've talked about with Eddie,

developed a a company called posit science where developed these brain training games that that can help improve specific cognitive functions and they're very easy to do because they're online and they you know they're there's science behind them and and you can you can do them so that in that way you don't have a therapist in your room but you can online sort of

do these sort of things that are targeting specific mechanisms to try to improve the kind of things that we think are impaired by concussion. And I'd like to see more patients get started on some of those things. Importantly, if you go on the web and just say, I will do brain training, you'll be overwhelmed with things and you don't know what works and what doesn't work. Yeah. I think the work that Merzenich and colleagues have done, and we'll provide a link to that. I don't have any financial stake in his work or products, trainings, that is.

But I will say, I think Mike's work has been tremendous. I mean, he is so far ahead of the curve. 20 years ago, everyone was talking about neuroplasticity in critical periods. They gave a Nobel Prize to it, to my scientific great-grandparents, David Hubel and Torrenson Wiesel.

And they deserved that Nobel Prize. But there was a kind of a central tenet of neuroscience at that time was that critical period plasticity ends around adolescence or one's early 20s. And that is simply not true. And Merzenich really, I think, is one of the people who deserves credit for making it clear that plasticity is ongoing. It takes some focus and work to access it in adulthood, but that we can all access neuroplasticity. But it takes...

It's there. So I don't know. They should give Merzenich a Nobel too, but you know, I'm not on the committee. So just a little editorial there. The description of specific cognitive trainings that can improve working memory in people that have had traumatic brain injury or concussion, as well as our earlier discussion about the development of frontal lobe function and plasticity of frontal lobe function makes me wonder,

is the working memory circuitry and frontal lobe function, a use it or lose it kind of circuit. Meaning if somebody goes to high school, graduates high school, and then gets into a lifestyle or college and then graduates college as well, and then gets into a lifestyle where they're not reading very many books, they're definitely scrolling social media, they're carrying out their daily tasks with apparently a high degree of functionality.

but they're not really pushing these forebrain circuits. Do we imagine that some of those forebrain circuits regress, aka use it or lose it? It seems to me that a few years back, maybe 10, 15 years back, there was a lot of interest in how to maintain cognitive function. In fact, one of the most common questions I would get, even as a neuroscientist primarily focused on the visual and autonomic nervous system, was how do I keep my memory as I age?

It seems to me that training it up and then continuing to use those circuits would be a really good way. Reading books without forcing oneself to finish the chapter, even though distractions jump into one's head, things like that.

For me, it's when I go to the gym, I try not to bring my phone. And if I do, I'll listen to one album of music, but I won't allow myself to play on my phone. I try. I mean, not interrupting a conversation with text messaging. I mean, basically the landscape I'm trying to draw here is it seems like the world is designed to disrupt, the modern world is designed to disrupt working memory and cognition of the frontal lobes. And we need to do some real training, just like

muscles and atrophy and cardiac fitness atrophies if we're not doing resistance and cardiovascular training.

Yeah, I think that's fair. I think, you know, of all the systems that decline with aging, not every brain system declines, but certainly the frontal executive system we're talking about is one that takes more of a decline than others. That's just how it is with healthy aging. Not surprising. It's the most complicated system and it's probably the most biologically costly. And so, you know, the more complicated system is going to

take more of a hit than other systems. And so certainly, I don't know about regressing, but certainly we're not, we may be accelerating this decline that we know exists. But a way I would think about it though is that not just trying to prevent a decline, but what we talked about before, there's no reason not to optimize. If everything is couched and I don't want to get dementia and don't want to get Alzheimer's disease and I don't want to get this and that,

I think that's not the way we should be looking about it. We want to look about optimizing health and brain health and getting up to our optimal levels because otherwise we're always playing defense instead of playing offense. And that's really hard for neurologists. We have a hard time thinking about brain health even though we're the brain specialists. We think about brain disease.

And we're just now as a field start thinking about preventative neurology and thinking about it not just like stopping Alzheimer's disease but promoting health in a healthy brain.

And neurologists don't talk to patients about sort of healthy patients about being healthier. I love how candid you are about the medical profession. And I like to think it's changing. I don't know, something happened in the 2020, 2021 era. I feel, this is just my bias, but I feel that the general public started becoming more aware of the things they might do to support their mental and physical health.

Maybe they had more time on their hands, but I think there was just more foraging for information. I love the idea that through simple practices like forcing oneself to read a book chapter start to finish without looking at one's phone, even if it takes twice as long as one would like, redirecting one's focus when focus moves away is a way of keeping working memory and cognitive function alive.

maybe even strengthening it, as you said, optimizing it. You know, I think that there –

so much emphasis now on physical health, which I think is great. Sleep, thanks to Matt Walker, you know, he really, you know, brought that torch in on sleep. And now others like myself are really, you know, trying to amplify the message of the critical role of sleep, but also, you know, most people realize they should probably at least walk as much, you know, the 10,000 steps thing is not a bad idea. Getting some heart rate up a few times a week or more, maybe doing some resistance training a few times a week or more. And then, and not just for athletes, but for

elderly folks, men and women, you know, I feel like we need the same for cognition, for brain function. And there just isn't a structure to that. No one can say right now, you know, you need to do three chapters of reading, you know, fiction per week, or you got to read a, you got to learn a few new vocabulary words and then write sentences with them. They do it in school, but then we're just, you know, set into the general population and

-Right. -And most people I think regress, right? Yeah, I mean, I think the big problem with brain health is trying to have a measure of what brain health is. And it's interesting to me again as a physician thinking about it from a neurologist standpoint. When you go to your family doctor, your primary care physician every year from your yearly physical,

They examine every organ in your body except your brain, your lungs, your heart, your skull system, your skin, but what do they do for you? Outside of having a conversation with you... Yeah, no cognitive task. There's nothing... No working memory task. They don't measure your brain at all. And it's not their fault. We haven't provided... The field has not provided them with a test of brain health. Right. And so part of the problem is we don't have a measurement of brain health. I'm involved...

and it's something called the Brain Health Project, which is at UT Dallas, which is their goal as a study to enroll 100,000 people in... and they've been developing a brain health index. And that's a complicated thing to do, but I really believe they're onto something because it's not just cognition, it's cognition, it's social, it's lifestyle like sleep and it's...

its well-being. A brain health index is going to cover all of these aspects. So they've developed quite an interesting, important index which does try to capture all aspects of brain health and then can be used to track where you can track your brain health over time with interventions that they've developed. So we need something like... Once we develop a brain health index, then we have something to

to follow and to be able to measure if we are optimizing our brain health. Otherwise, how do you know if you're optimizing your brain health? Your doctor's not telling you, you don't know. All these games you get on in the web don't really tell you. So when we develop that, then all of the things that can promote brain health will be measurable and I think it will take off the way physical fitness did. Perhaps you get enough of it from your work

But given what you know about brain health and approaches to brain health, what are some of the things that you do besides sleep, exercise, nutrition, in terms of trying to

optimized brain function. I mean, do you make it a point to read fiction? Do you make it a point to learn new, new skills like instruments, things like that? Again, maybe your profession and your, and your personal life keeps you busy enough that you don't have to do those things. I mean, for me, I've gathering, organizing and disseminating the information for the podcast feels like the, the, the heavy lifting mental work for me, but, but I'm keenly aware of the fact that were I to read more fiction or learn an instrument, I,

I mean, everyone else around me would suffer if I learned an instrument, but that it would probably benefit me in some real way. What are the things that you do and that you think are kind of access points that hopefully people also enjoy? Yeah, no, I agree with that. I think when you have a busy career and you're doing many different things like teaching and research and seeing patients, I've always felt that I've

I'm maxing out on, I'm full of my executive functions being tested to the limit. But you're like an athlete, you're like a professional athlete of the mind. Yeah, in a way. But then, then you realize there's, that's, that's not, you know, that's not everything. There's so many other aspects. Everything emanates from the brain. So you start to think about what should I be doing in my, my life as a father and a husband? What, what, what should I be doing, you know, in terms of promoting social interactions with friends and, and,

and what should I be doing for sleep and nutrition. And it's funny you bring up books. I think I went probably 20 years where I never read any fictional book and said this can't be good for my brain and then just consciously started reading.

reading books and reading more nonfiction books and just- - Listening to books or reading hard books? - Reading, yeah. I still like to read the hard covers. - Yeah, likewise. - Unfortunately, when I was an undergraduate, with pre-med, they don't let you take any courses that are interesting. So I never learned any history or all the books that I never read. I started reading my kids

you know, English literature books that I never got a chance to and started reading history. And so, yeah, I always felt just like just increasing, you know, just knowledge was, you know, like I said, our brain just stores information. That's one of its,

this job, so that's got to be useful. But again, all of these things that I do believe help brain health, we need some way to measure it. I think certainly if you feel healthy that's an important thing. If you feel like you're healthy physically or mentally that's a good start. But if we actually had a way to sort of track it the way we can track heart disease,

lung disease and skin and things like that. I think that will really boost everyone's confidence that this is really making a difference. On the basis of today's conversation, I'm going to read some fiction. I read a lot of nonfiction and I enjoy it. I listen to some audio books, but it's crazy to me because I'm a neuroscientist by training and I understand neuroplasticity.

And I do know a bit about fitness and the key role of remaining active, kind of use it or lose it and maybe improve it. Right. Kind of, that wasn't intended to rhyme folks.

but clearly based on everything you've told us that if we don't exercise these working memory and other circuits for cognition, like why should they stick around? And I'm beginning to think that social media is entertaining as it is. And I learn there and I teach there, but that it's not a cohesive plot, right? It's like those baby otters,

That really cool looking dog that I'd love to own. I'm thinking about getting another dog. This interesting conference, you know, it's like random, pseudo random tailored to me, of course, because that's what their algorithms do. But that's not following a plot, character development, antagonist, protagonist, you

any of the things that provide cognitive richness. It's kind of obvious as I say this, but I feel like we're divorced from these things that really helped evolve culture and evolved individuals. And it takes some discipline, but...

like a run or going to the gym, you do a few times for a shorter amount of time with less intensity and pretty soon you're up to speed and there's an upper threshold unless you're going to be a pro. I'm not, I'm certainly not going to be an English lit, you know, professor. You know, so I don't think, obviously boosting executive function is incredibly important, but I don't think it's going to happen just with technology. I think it's going to need, it needs human interaction. I mean, I, I believe executive function should be taught in, you know, school as one of the core, you know, as, as a course. Um,

This goal management theory that I talk about could be taught in school. It's what you do with your students. For example, when you have a graduate student and they have to learn how to read the literature and design an experiment and carry out the experiment, there's no technology that's going to just be able to teach them how to do that. You've got to intervene sometimes and say,

stop reading all those papers, they're not relevant, okay? Or you piloted this enough, get going. It's that kind of wisdom that you get when you get older that has to be on top of the technology. So that's why I think it needs to be directed. So whether it's in school

Or whether it's in a patient, I think there still needs to be someone coaching us, you know? And I know that's why life coaches have been, some people have really benefited from life coaches because someone just, it sounds obvious when you tell your kids, you know,

Just write, you know, just do it this way, you know, break it up into little pieces. It seems so obvious, but to them it's not always obvious and they just need to be told something simple for it to make a big difference. And in school we were brought along step by step and there was context. So why wouldn't it be the same in adulthood? I'm realizing I should probably learn how to play chess. Yeah.

- Seems like a good game, right? - Chess, yeah, any one of these. - Working memory, you gotta keep information online. There's a bunch else there, I think, but as a tool to improve cognition. I was also thinking, you know, some people orient more towards the arts. My sister is really great about,

She's a therapist in San Francisco, but also takes like theater classes. And she said, you know, like improv is like forces you to keep on your toes, keep the context there. You're up on your feet. Like, you know, and I wasn't a theater kid. I did the crew. I did like the pulling the curtains and stuff till I went and did other things. But that whole biz is about...

Being, you know, learning the novel rule set in the moment, you know, improv by definition. Absolutely. I mean, anything that requires you to have that, you know, where there's a goal and you've got to break it down into sub goals and you've got to do it simultaneously and you got to filter out distractions. And, you know, you know, for example, my, my kids got me one of these pizza ovens for, for, uh,

Christmas. And you know, you think it's easy to sort of make some pizza and throw it in the oven and done. That's where I'm on my third time and it's still not even round. I'll come over and test. That sounds good. That sounds good. And you're in a city with great food. So the standard is really high. I'm tempted to make a reference to the cheese board pizza, but I want to keep the lines as short as possible because they're already too long. Um,

Maybe we could talk a little bit about some other unfortunately common disease states. Parkinson's and Alzheimer's. Let's start with Alzheimer's. I think few things scare people more than the idea of getting Alzheimer's, especially if they have Alzheimer's in their family. First of all, what is the genetic link with Alzheimer's? If one has a parent or grandparent that got Alzheimer's, is there a known increase in one's risk for getting Alzheimer's?

It's not that straightforward as other diseases. There's diseases like Huntington's disease where it's a very strong link that if you have a parent, you have a very high chance of getting it. But there's so many factors that it's not necessarily the case that you're increased your risk of getting it. There are families where there is something special about the family where it just runs in families. But I try to not scare people.

my patient's children into worrying that they're necessarily going to get Alzheimer's because it's not that straightforward. As I understand, Alzheimer's is a neurodegenerative disorder, impacts the hippocampus, among other structures. There's been some debate in recent years as to whether or not the whole amyloid hypothesis is real or not. There's a bunch of, unfortunately, false data accusations and that whole thing. But

My understanding is that if you look at a slice of human brain from a patient that died with Alzheimer's, maybe even from Alzheimer's, that you see plaques and tangles. You see these subcellular structures and buildup. And that our basic understanding of Alzheimer's that's in the textbook and that most people have heard of is

is still correct, right? - Yes. - Okay, because I think a couple of years ago, it was, unfortunately the way social media sometimes can work is that, you know, the idea was that it was all wrong, you know, all wrong. And indeed somebody fudged data, they made up data and that's terrible.

But Alzheimer's is an neurodegenerative disorder, includes the hippocampus. Plaques and tangles are present in the neurons. Those are not good for neurons, as I understand. So what's the controversy? And why don't we have a treatment for Alzheimer's yet? I feel like almost every other psychiatric disease, including Parkinson's, there are certain things you can do to at least push the system in the right way. Why is Alzheimer's and other dementias so tricky?

Yeah, I mean it's very frustrating because the neurodegenerative disorders, it's just so many factors that are probably involved in the pathology that there's not one single transmitter. In Parkinson's disease it's a decreased dopamine and so one transmitter can make a very big difference. Early on in Alzheimer's it was discovered that there was low acetylcholine in the brains and the only approved

a treatment for Alzheimer's disease is a drug that boosts acetylcholine. What's the drug? It's called the Nepazil. There's a few of them. They're anticholinesterase inhibitors that boost acetylcholine. They've been around for 20 years or more. And, you know, the reality is when you give it to your patients, they don't see much of a difference because it's not the primary deficit. So the real problem has been trying to find out what is the primary mechanism that's leading to this.

the wide range of cognitive behavioral issues and there doesn't seem to be at least one neurotransmitter that can make the difference. And so now the push has been is there one, is there something else that we can do? Can we block amyloid? Can we block something in the pathology? And again it just has not been successful. It's very frustrating because I think it was over probably 35 years ago I saw my first Alzheimer's patients and I don't believe I saved that much different

to them now, you know, except that we have a lot more things we can do just on the social side of things. But unfortunately for drugs we don't have anything that's been really transformative. But again that's I think part of a... and being a neurologist it sounds very depressing but I think part of what the family isn't always looking for a cure, of course that's they'd like to have a cure but I think

them understanding what's going on, what to expect, how to handle the behaviors is what they're really after, at least until we find a cure. Parkinson's, you mentioned, is a deficit in dopaminergic function due largely to degeneration of dopaminergic neurons. There, there's some effective treatments, right? L-DOPA. Did you know there's this over-the-counter stuff that's sold that a lot of people take who don't have Parkinson's? I'm not suggesting they take it, called mucunipurines.

It's the velvety bean. Yeah, I've heard of it. It's 99% L-dopa. I've heard of it, yeah. It's present in like some energy drinks and supplements. I mean, people can go buy it. I'm not suggesting they do that. I actually tried it. Boy, being really dopamined out does not, to me, doesn't feel that good. Yeah, I felt kind of agitated. My vision got a little, you know, twinkly. It did not feel like a high of any kind. And then I felt lousy for a couple hours after it wore off. Yeah, I don't think you can really...

get in enough L-DOPA to get enough into your brain. That happened early in neurology when it was discovered we couldn't give our patients enough L-DOPA without them feeling bad because it's also metabolized in the periphery. And so it wasn't until we, Sinemet came along, which has this deboxylase, decarboxylase inhibitor that blocks sort of the

the breakdown of dopamine, that we were able to sort of get enough dopamine into the brain. So I'm not sure, yeah, so that's why I think it's not going to probably get the levels up high enough in the brain. So Parkinson's patients are given L-DOPA

Or bromocriptine or drugs like that. Going back to Alzheimer's for a moment, I mean, what do you tell somebody who has early stage Alzheimer's? You just say, listen, try and get good sleep, try and keep people around you, stay cognitively engaged, try and keep those circuits going through behavioral induced neuroplasticity. But we're just going to watch the steepness of the decline.

Is that really all we've got? All we've got is to help them with everything that comes up on a day-to-day basis. A lot of the problems, the memory problems tend to be something that families can help compensate for, but you do get to a point where you can't be with someone for 24-7. It's a real burnout for caregivers. A lot of the behaviors that come up, patients get kind of delusions and agitated and

and some of the medications that we use for other conditions are helpful for treating that. But it's really just a purely symptomatic therapy. And the more socialization that patients get, they tend to do better. There was a study back at Penn way back that if you showed patients

family movies or family albums, it was better than any drug you could give them to sort of help their behavior. So there's still those memories are in there and they were making some type of contact that was helping them emotionally that you couldn't turn off with a drug. So I think the more we do things like that, the more will be helpful for them, at least symptomatically.

I've seen a number of videos on social media of people with Alzheimer's who listen to a piece of music or people with Parkinson's that hear a piece of music and that seems to resurrect some at least context-appropriate emotional state where it kind of brings the person to the surface again. Yeah, it's kind of a tragic situation for Alzheimer's right now. It seems like if ever there was a call to arms for the neurology community and

biotech and behavioral tech would be for Alzheimer's, for the treatment of Alzheimer's. Yeah, absolutely. I will never ask a guest to comment on the good or bad behaviors of other people except my own. But there's a Nobel Prize winning neuroscientist and I visited him. He's at a big East Coast school and

back in 2010. And during the course of our one hour meeting, he consumed no fewer than four pieces of Nicorette gum. And I said, I got to ask, what's this about? By the way, he's extremely sharp still. And he said, oh yeah, yeah, yeah. You know, I used to be a smoker, but smoking is really bad for you because you can get lung cancer.

dipping's bad for you because you can get mouth cancer, but nicotine, these are his words, by the way, is protective against Parkinson's and Alzheimer's and it keeps my brain sharp. So I chew Nicorette all day long. And I thought, okay, well, he is an MD actually.

And I thought, well, that's interesting. And I did an episode of this podcast on nicotine. By the way, it can raise blood pressure. It's certainly smoking, vaping, dipping or snuffing, not good, bad, don't do it. But there is some interest in the use of nicotine as a cognitive enhancer. So I'd love to know your thoughts on that. And I'd love to know your thoughts on his statement about

being a potential way to stave off Parkinson's and Alzheimer's with the caveat that he just kind of threw that out there. And this guy's sort of known for just kind of throwing stuff out there every once in a while. I have a feeling, you know who this person is, but in any event,

What gives? Yeah, well, I don't know anything about nicotine staving off any neurodegenerative disorder. But nicotine was used in a number of early Alzheimer's studies just because of its effect on the cholinergic system. So there is some...

truth to that, uh, the cholinergic system is, is, uh, you know, dysfunctional in Alzheimer's disease and boosting the cholinergic system probably is beneficial. I mean, the patients that we give the anticholinergic inhibitors, there are some families that say, yeah, he just, he's, he's remembering more and he's just doing better. So they're, they're, I've seen positive, um,

things to it. It doesn't really slow the course of the disease. That's the problem. The disease just carries on even though we're symptomatically improving the symptoms. But again, I think it's going to take both acetylcholine and something else. I think we don't know, should we give dopamine with the nicotine or the acetylcholine? Should we give norepinephrine? I think it's going to be a cocktail, which again, pharmaceutical companies, you know, have not tried a cocktail of neuromodulators for Alzheimer's disease. They've just tried acetylcholine.

Sounds like you should be running the FDA. No disrespect to the current people in charge, by the way. But maybe, actually, I'm a big believer that there shouldn't be individuals in charge of large organizations. There should be panels. I mean, there's so much talk about diversity, but they appoint individuals. You can't get it right. Anytime there's, when there's only one person, by definition. So committees, folks, committees. Anyway, another editorial.

Are there any sex differences, male-female differences in sort of these dopamine levels, working memory, injuries, concussion, like things that would direct people toward different routes of treatment given that, you know, maybe there's more susceptibility in one case, maybe less susceptibility, maybe certain neurotransmitters are more effective in improving symptoms in men versus women. Do you see that in the clinic?

Yeah, that's a great question. There was Emily Jacobs, who's a professor at UC Santa Barbara now in the psychology department, when she was a graduate student in my lab, studied the role of estrogen on working memory and dopaminergic function.

And what she brought to my attention at the time, and it was embarrassing that I didn't know, was that the frontal lobes are full of estrogen receptors. There's probably more estrogen receptors in the frontal lobes than any other part of the brain. In men and women? Estrogen boosts dopamine. So higher estrogen levels correlates with increased dopamine.

levels and there was some anecdotal evidence that in postmenopausal women who were put on estrogen that their working memory improved and there was a kind of evolving link between estrogen and frontal lobe function and she did this amazing study

where she studied healthy Berkeley undergraduates at two points in time during their menstrual cycle when estrogen was at its lowest and what was its highest. And she also genotyped them for this enzyme they were talking about to know if they were sort of lower or higher on the dopamine level.

And then put them in the scanner and measured frontal lobe function and showed that there was a clear frontal lobe function was modulated by where they were in their estrogen cycle. When they were low estrogen, they were low dopamine. And if they were low estrogen and low dopamine to start, they were really...

had decreased frontal lobe function and decreased working memory ability. So it fluctuated based on this interaction between estrogen and dopamine, suggesting that, you know, not only dopamine is important but hormones are clearly important and they work synergistically. So, you know, as we're developing this cocktail,

We certainly have to bring hormones into the equation and learn more about it. There's just so little information about hormones and cognition. Yeah, one of the themes that's come out of some of the episodes we've done with MDs who specialize in endocrine health is that for both men and for women,

optimizing estrogen levels is really important for cognition and vascular function. I think people mistakenly hear, okay, testosterone men, you know, estrogen women, obviously both hormones are present in men and women. In fact, I think, um, I know that testosterone levels in women are actually higher than their estrogen levels. When you look at, when you sort of, um,

use the same units of measure. It just so happens that they still have lower testosterone on average than the typical male, but that men whose estrogen levels are too low suffer cognitive defects and vascular defects. So this idea that more testosterone, lower estrogen in men is the ideal. And

It just doesn't hold. It doesn't hold. I mean, unless you want to be dumb and have a heart attack. It just doesn't hold. Very interesting. Do we know what estrogen is doing there? Is it specifically raising dopamine? We don't have to get into the synaptic biology, but it's so interesting. Yeah, it's actually boosting dopamine activity.

So it's making more dopamine available. Yeah, yeah. It's really amazing. And to think about it sort of fluctuating, certainly during the menstrual cycle we can think about how much it fluctuates in an injured woman over 30 days, but then you can think across individuals, you can think about how much it can account for individual differences. So not only sort of knowing your dopamine level, but just knowing sort of estrogen and serotonin is really going to be important.

Interesting. Is there any evidence that physical exercise can improve working memory and cognition separate from the known improvements in cardiovascular function and blood flow to the brain that occur with exercise? Like, is there anything about going for, you know, a 45 minute bout of exercise, you know, pick your favorite exercise, and then doing cognitive work?

immediately afterward when presumably the catecholamines, dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine are going to be circulating, at least in the blood, at higher levels? Has that stuff ever been explored? In all of the groups around the country that are trying to develop cognitive therapies, they often use aerobic exercise as another type of therapy. So for example, the group at University of Illinois,

Champaign-Arkhamer's group has done aerobic exercise quite a bit and they can find it just as effective as cognitive therapy and improving executive function, just straight up. Wow.

wow aerobic exercise wow and um and so you know the hard part in the real world is is how do you get you know a seven year eight year old you know do the kind of aerobics that you're but but now with you know recumbent bicycles and now there has been studies with seven year olds with just putting them in mostly with recumbent bicycles it's sort of designing we have to think about ways to design exercise that can get aerobics up but it's it's uh

And you know, neurologists are starting-- I think my field is starting to realize that we got to tackle this every way we can. And so now I'm hearing more neurologists talk about that-- 30 years ago no neurologist would say you got to exercise more, you know, now it's talking about exercise and nutrition and sleep and it's all becoming sort of part of our package of how we're going to help our patients, but the aerobic exercise

is super interesting and I think it's going to be... You know, that kind of made me think that what we didn't talk about was mindfulness. And so when we add... A lot of these studies also if they add mindfulness training to the hardcore goal management training, it's better than just the executive training alone. Just learning skills to stop, relax, refocus.

kind of gives this sort of boost to executive function as well. Yeah, I think of mindfulness like sort of, well, there's no such thing as traditional meditation. I have to be careful here. But the sort of stereotypical meditation of closing one's eyes, closing one's eyes, excuse me, sitting down, lying down and just...

focusing on one's breath and then redirecting one's focus to maybe third eye center, you know, area between the forehead, just redirecting focus, redirecting. I think of meditation of that sort as a focus exercise, right? It's, it's not so much a perceptual exercise because thoughts are kind of, you know, doing what they're doing. Um,

It's like a focus exercise. And that's half of the problem with not achieving our goals, right, is we lose our focus. And so building into sort of strategies to main focus, you know, to stop and relax and refocus is an important strategy for boosting executive function. And it doesn't seem to matter what, you know,

I know there's all different flavors of mindfulness. So we just happened to use one when we were studying it. It doesn't yet, I don't think we know enough about how we should tailor the mindfulness, but most forms of mindfulness will work of the type you're talking about. That's similar. What I described is similar to what you guys explored. Yeah. I mean, it's amazing to me, you know,

20 years ago, if somebody wanted to talk about neuroscience and mindfulness on a major university campus, let's say Stanford or Berkeley was probably a little bit more tolerant of these ideas at that time, just given the kind of culture. Yeah.

they wouldn't have been laughed out of the room, but there was a lot of skepticism. And I feel like now mindfulness meditation, breath work, the idea that, oh my goodness, breathing can impact your emotional state. I mean, that should have been obvious. But now that people are on board then, now of course there's a lot of interest in psychedelics. That's kind of a new emerging therapy carrying more risk, potential risk, but it looks like it's

very likely that some of those are going to make it through the, the FDA filters at some point. But the conversation we're having now, you know, neurologist, neurobiologist talking about mindfulness, nutrition, we're talking freely about nicotine. You know, we're not suggesting people do that. Bromocriptine as a, to optimize cognitive function. I mean, this conversation would have never happened. Um,

seven years ago. No, it's just... The field has changed. Yeah. I hear neurologists talk about it all the time. So do you try mindfulness? And if you do, does it make your day? Do you feel like you perform better that day? Yeah. Thanks for asking. There are two forms of yes. The short answer is yes. There's a very specific practice that I've used since 2017 that's really benefited me so much, which is what I call it non-sleep deep rest, but it's based on a practice called yoga nidra, where you just lie down

And these are free audio scripts online. We can provide links to these. And you go through a body scan and you do some long exhale breathing, which emphasizes the parasympathetic, aka relaxing aspect of the autonomic nervous system. I know you know this, I'm saying that for the audience. And it does involve some intentions and things like that, but it can also just be...

self-directed relaxation. And I emerged from that with much more mental and physical vigor than I did prior. And this only takes maybe 10 minutes. There's also a 30 minute scripts. I do those. And then I do mindfulness meditation. The thing about mindfulness meditation that the biggest impact for me has been the problems of my life get re-

I get a different perspective. I start thinking about things in different domains of time. Like this thing that is like a problem that I've been dealing with, for instance, I start thinking, you know, like in the course of my lifetime, this is, you know, a relatively small, not small thing, but in relatively small time bin. And I sort of think about, you know, best course of action given its real role in my life and what I want, et cetera. So I feel like it sort of orients me in time. So that's been a major effect.

For focus, the best tool I know is to, you know,

put the phone in another room. That's kind of a don't. And I know our friend Eddie Chang, neurosurgeon, chair of neurosurgery at UCSF, he's big on mindfulness meditation. So do you meditate? - No, I think that's one of the things when we're talking about what should we do besides reading fiction, I think that should be on my list. - Five minutes a day. - No, I guess it's just amazing that the patients tell me about it and what we've seen from our studies.

You know, a lot of this like again I was saying before is if we had some measure of brain health that we could see the impact of it would sort of push us towards probably doing it more. I think another thing that we didn't sort of talk about, we talked a little about what dopamine is, are there other kind of brain states that sort of predict how you're going to respond to these therapies and if you're going to benefit from them.

We've done a lot of work with measuring the large-scale organization of the brain and brain networks, and that's a very popular idea in neuroscience today, moving away from what is this. We've talked a lot about what the frontal lobes do, but the frontal lobes are part of these networks in the brain. And really, the state of your networks is a really important factor as well, in addition to your neurochemical networks.

Yeah, tell me more about this. I mean, you actually...

Preempted my next question, which is going to be, and this is my favorite question to ask. Carl DeSaroff taught me to ask this way back when. Like, what are you most excited about now? Because I know the work you've published and you've done a magnificent job of sharing the details of that and work of others in a really informative and in some cases actionable way. But what are you really excited about? Like if there were no...

financial barriers to your grants, et cetera. You had a thousand people working. What's the thing that's hitting your dopamine circuits these days? Well, in the grand scale, I'm excited that things that we've learned over the last 30 years, not just in my lab or your lab or anyone's lab, is actually now being translated to actually helping people. I mean, when people ask me what I do, I say I'm a neurologist because that's at my core what I feel I am. And I feel I got into this business to

They help people. And so it's when you work for years and years and years and it doesn't translate, it can be frustrating. But now I'm excited that it seems that the things we've learned, that all of us have learned in neuroscience is starting to now translate into something else.

In neuroscience what's happened in the last 10 years is we're thinking of the brain in a grander scale, it's overall organization and not so focused on just this area or that area. When I talk about the frontal lobes as being the most important part, the conductor,

Yes, I am talking about one brain region, but it's a brain region, like I said, that's connected to everywhere. And it's because it's connected to everywhere is what's really the essence of why it's so important. So some of the research I'm excited most about is sort of taking away the names of areas and just thinking about the brain as a big network, like an airline network or electrical network and how...

different areas communicate with each other and when you think of it that way so for example an airline network you've got all these hubs all over the world all over the country in the united states for example you've got chicago is is a hub and and there's other hud milwaukee or cincinnati but they have very different functions in the network as a whole right if you're trying to get from

New York to San Francisco, which happened to me many times, even though you're not going through Chicago, if Chicago is shut down, you're probably going to get delayed because it just has this huge impact on the whole system.

And, you know, if Milwaukee goes down, you don't even know it. You just fly right over there. I'm sorry if anyone's listening from Milwaukee. There are probably a few. You got to go through. But so thinking about the brain, the brain is the same way. The brain has these hubs as well. And, you know, the prefrontal cortex is a hub like Chicago. It's just an important hub that keeps the whole system going. And that's why it has much more of an impact when you –

when you damage it or you stress it as opposed to some other part of the brain. And so what's exciting to me is not only is that making us thinking about disease differently, because now we're starting to think about how is diseases affecting these hubs, that the pathology seems to be like when you look at Alzheimer's disease and you look at schizophrenia and you look at a lot of diseases, it's not just that there's some microscopic change in some neuron, it seems to be affecting

in the brain that are affecting the whole network. And so we have a different target to go after for treatments. What can we do to sort of boost that?

you know, a hub that's been damaged as opposed to thinking about it in a less specific way. And then also as we really start to learn about how the brain is organized in these networks, we've also learned that measuring your network structure is very predictive of your well-being and how you respond to interventions. So there's a metric called modularity.

which measures how organized your brain networks are. And the brain is made up of different modules, different networks, and these networks can either be very communicating with each other or not so communicating with each other. And the more segregated they are, we call that more modularity. They're kind of separate entities. They're modular. And it turns out we can measure that with fMRI. We can put someone in a scanner. We can do this resting process.

fMRI scanning and then we can measure how modular your brain is versus my brain and all of us are very

different levels of modularity. Is it more advantageous to have more modularity as opposed to less? Yeah, it turns out that it seems to be more advantageous to have more. So we can predict... More separateness of brain function between areas. Yeah, that the networks are sort of more independent. That doesn't mean they don't talk to each other, but at sort of baseline, they're more independent. Resting state connectivity. Yeah, they're more independent as opposed to...

less independent with each other. Not unlike neuromuscular junctions. During development are what we call polyinnervated. That's why babies can move their limbs, but not with a lot of coordination. Look at a

One-year-old trying to eat spaghetti, for instance, it's hilarious. Look at that same kid seven months later, there's a lot more precision of movement largely due to removal, more modularity of connections. Right, right. Interesting. So we did a study where we took 12 traumatic brain injury patients and measured their modularity. So you get a number, you know, you just get a modularity index for each of the 12 people. And then they underwent this goal management training and we were able to predict who was going to improve

on the training. Those who had more, started off more modular, better.

benefited more from the training. And it's turned out that this has been a very robust finding across studies now, across different training, different young, old patient populations. It's also predicted things like whether someone in a coma is more likely to do well or if someone who's older is going to have a certain amount of cognitive decline or someone's going to respond to behavioral therapy if they're obsessive-compulsive. So there's something about this brain state

that not only we can measure, but actually is giving us insight into the interventions that we're doing, which again is going to be much more helpful in determining what helps and what doesn't help if we know sort of what the state is before we start the intervention. So interesting and makes me think many things, but...

I'll just focus on two of them. One is I love this idea that you and others are starting to look at brain network activity as opposed to overemphasizing the role of, say, prefrontal cortex or hippocampus, understanding more that those are hubs in a larger theme of

of activation. Because, you know, if I had one wish for science communication, it's that people would, yes, learn some terms like dopamine and frontal lobes. It is important to know the nomenclature, but to understand that if you really want to be able to work with the information in a way that's beneficial, you need to think about verbs, not nouns. Right. It's about the action states of these areas. And those action states are always, um,

involving multiple areas. Just like you can't talk about running as just like quadricep and hamstring flexion and extension and contraction. You can, you can break it down that way. And it's useful to know that, but ultimately you're talking about gait and stride and things that have a real verb action to them. And we haven't had so much of that for the nervous system at a circuit level. We've been able to do that for individual neurons. That's the first piece.

And then the second piece is that, you know, it occurs to me that there's so much rich understanding of the different states of sleep. You know, Matt Walker was just here recording this series on sleep that we'll release later this year. And your stage is one, two, three, four, deep sleep, slow wave sleep, rapid eye movement sleep. But we don't even really have a naming system for waking states. Like we say focus, we say task switching, but those are

just names we made up, just as stage one, two, three and REM sleep are names that we made up. But there seems to be a much richer understanding of like what rapid eye movement sleep is good for and what deficits in rapid eye movement sleep lead to. Then there is, for instance, how given our network, I'm going to make this up, like calling a certain network activation state, like state A,

Like we, I feel like neuroscience is tasked, the field of neuroscience now is tasked with giving us an understanding of the verb states and like what, like these waking states of mind are very mysterious. And for the general public, this is important because people wonder like, is my focus poor or is it good? Is my task switching ability good? I mean, we only tend to look at, you know, are they functional enough?

to do their job and manage their family, manage their lives. We don't really have metrics, but for sleep, we have metrics and commercial products can measure that, you know, sleep tracker rings, wristbands, mattress covers that, I mean,

this sort of thing. Well, you know, yeah, I think modularity can actually be that metric, some metric of your large-scale organization of your brain can be that metric. There's a number of labs that have done this, have measured modularity in real time. So what I was talking about, we're just getting a snapshot of this is what your baseline modularity is. But we can also look at modularity as changes on a second-to-second-to-minute-to-minute basis. And

One of my former postdocs, Sepera Saragiani, she just did a very simple experiment where there were sounds and the functional MRI scanner is very loud so you can't hear very well but every once in a while there'd be a sound that was just above the level of the noise of the scanner and all you had to do was sort of press a button if you heard that sound. And you didn't pick it up all the time, maybe 80% of the time you heard it and

sometimes 20% of the times you didn't hear it. Well, she measured their modularity on a moment-to-moment basis, and she could predict if they were going to get that, if they were going to be correct or not and wreck the sound before they got the sound. If they were highly modular...

Boom, they got it. If their brain had gone into this kind of diffuse, less moderate state, they missed it. And so I could definitely see, as you're talking about, where if we could develop a modularity metric in real time,

on a device, this would be game changer. And so, and that's sort of what I'm, you know, what I've been interested in doing. What excites me is that we're not going to do with a scanner. Obviously, you can't walk around with a scanner in your head, right? And even, I don't think you could even do with the EEG. I think, can we develop a proxy for modularity with some more simple software?

of doing it? Can we extract this maybe out of heart rate variability or for oxygen? I've been working with some colleagues. They were a former student, Brian Miller and a postdoc of Adam Ghazali's West Clap who have a company called Neuroscouting

where they are able to-- they have-- we've been sort of doing scanning and also collecting physiological data to try and determine if there's some-- we can measure the modularity in the scanner but can we pick that up in the physiology data because they can collect, you know, oxygen and heart rate variability and other metrics that may be kind of a readout of that. And then we'd have a brain state which is what you were looking-- you know, you're looking for some brain state but it's not--

I think people are thinking we need a helmet or something like that. We need just something simple, right, that reads out brain state just the way we read out other physiological information from our watch or our

or something like that. Well, the sleep trackers of various kinds have certainly been able to pull out information about rapid eye movement and other stages of sleep. I mean, key metrics, not every metric and not what you would get with a person wearing a EEG, uh, you know, probe or something, you know, set of probes, but certainly information that can be used. Um, one thing that has me a little bit perplexed, um, and, uh,

I'm almost reluctant to bring it up, but I'm going to do it, is that I did a couple episodes about psilocybin and the use of psilocybin for the treatment of depression. This is Robin Cardart-Harris from UCSF, and I also did a solo episode emphasizing, of course, this isn't recreational use we're talking about. We're talking about for treatment of depression. But there's a lot of neuroimaging about psilocybin.

of patients before and after macrodose psilocybin, and this isn't microdosing. And one of the major takeaways is increased resting state connectivity, which by virtue of what you just described might not be ideal for cognitive function. It might be good for social-emotional function, and I certainly don't want to disparage the beautiful work that's being done there. But

You said that increased modularity predicted improved function, especially with cognitive interventions. Psilocybin seems to induce fairly significant increases in cross-modal talk between brain networks. In other words, less modularity. So should we be...

No, it's just it has to do with how we make these measurements and connectivity doesn't mean the same. You know, there's different types of connectivity. And so I like to when I think about connectivity, we talked about this connectivity of a brain state and versus a brain trait. So when we're talking about you being highly modular as a trait, right.

That's very different than what your modularity is like in different states. It actually turns out when you do these highly executive demanding tasks, you get less modular because your networks are communicating with each other. So it's important for networks to get less modular when it's a more demanding task.

But that's very different than what's your baseline modularity. Because you've got to get from where your baseline is to this other state. And a lot of it has to do with going from one state to another, not so much the absolute differences. So that's interesting. I didn't know about those results, but it's interesting that it does affect connectivity in that way. I think the drugs that are going to be helpful are going to,

are going to promote sort of networks talking to each other as opposed to networks not communicating with each other. In your clinic, do you ever combine drug therapies, cognitive training, and things like transcranial magnetic stimulation? Do you use stimulators? Yeah, so I think...

You know, I have a lot of patients that I've referred for its approved use, which is depression. So I'm very excited about sort of the work that's being done for depression. But we haven't really had any improved anything that's been for depression.

you know, for cognition. So there are a bunch of studies, anecdotal small studies, where you can give transcranial maxillofacial stimulation in the frontal cortex and working memory improved, but they really haven't been done in ways that are... we don't know if it generalizes, if it's going to be how, you know, the way it's been done in depression in a way that can really be... But again, it's just a matter of doing it. I think it will be part of the things we do, drugs, TMS,

and all the other things we've talked about, it's not just going to be one thing. And it gets back to networks, right? What this is doing is really changing how nodes, you know, the interaction of regions. It's not about sort of just increasing or decreasing activity in some mysterious part of the brain, it's just sort of restoring the balance of a network.

Well, Mark, I just really want to thank you. You, um, you're gonna given us an amazing tour of, uh, basically five fields. I threw a lot at you, um, you know, as a neurologist, but the way I'm, I'm slightly reluctant to do this, but, um, I'm going to tell you a joke, um, that was told to me, um, uh, so that there are these people stranded on an Island and, um,

they're really stuck and they're running out of resources. And by the way, this joke was told to me by a physician. And all of a sudden this hot air balloon then comes over and they're like, oh my goodness. So they start, they write help in the sand and they, you know, and hot air balloon directly over them kind of descends almost, you know, almost to them. And then someone in the hot air balloon says, you know,

i'm doing the measurement and it's exactly 76 feet down to those people and then the hot air balloon takes off and goes away and the people on the beach one of them's a physician and he goes those were neurologists i tell that joke because that was the old school view of neurology that neurologists were great at describing things talking about the terrible conditions they could observe in great detail but that they did not do anything about it

You, on the other hand, and I'm guessing others in the field,

But certainly you have proven today that that joke needs to be revised whereby there's one, at least one neurologist who casts a line down and shimmies down and assists them and pulls those stranded people on the island up to the balloon. Because today you've described the underlying nature of some of these things like working memory deficits, traumatic brain injury, concussion, Alzheimer's, Parkinson's. Again, I threw a lot at you and you responded with enthusiasm.

in thorough, clear detail, but also a number of things that clearly can assist in these situations, such as bromocriptine, mindfulness, exercise, and really as an exploration of what can be done, interventions. And so for all those reasons and for tolerating this terrible joke that I just told, I want to say thank you because I've learned a ton.

And I know the audience has learned a ton and much of what we've learned has us looking in the directions of possibility to alleviate these situations. And as you point out, for the already healthy even to optimize brain function and health. So for all of that, thanks for sliding down the rope to the island.

Well I'd say you know on behalf of all the neurologists in the world, thank you. We're appreciating what we do. It's just so important to try and get this message apart. Like I said you know with patients we just try to have them understand what it is that they're going through and I think today patients have to really be advocates for themselves and so I think the more they learn about all of these possibilities,

the more they can go back to their doctors or whoever and try and ask for, you know, what about this? What about that? Do you think this would help me? Because we have to be advocates for our own health. And the only way we're going to do that is just make people understand what it is that the possibilities are. So thank you. It was a lot of fun. It was a great time. Well, amen to all of that. And I hope to have you back again. Thanks so much. You're welcome.

Thank you for joining me for today's discussion about the brain mechanisms of cognition and memory and how to optimize cognition and memory with Dr. Mark Desposito. To learn more about Dr. Desposito's work, please see the links in the show note captions. If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on both Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple, you can leave us up to a five-star review.

Please check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this podcast.

If you have questions or comments about the podcast or topics or guests that you'd like to suggest for the Huberman Lab Podcast, please put those in the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments. Not so much on today's episode, but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we discuss supplements. While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like improving sleep, for improving hormone function, and for improving focus.

To learn more about the supplements discussed on the Huberman Lab podcast, visit LiveMomentous, spelled O-U-S, so that's livemomentous.com slash Huberman. If you're not already following me on social media, I am Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. So that's Instagram, Twitter, now called X, LinkedIn, Facebook, and Threads.

And on all those platforms, I discuss science and science-related tools, some of which overlaps with the content of the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the content covered on the Huberman Lab podcast. Again, that's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. If you haven't already subscribed to our monthly Neural Network newsletter,

Our Neural Network Newsletter is a zero-cost newsletter that includes podcast summaries and protocols as short one- to three-page PDFs. For instance, we have zero-cost protocols for improving sleep, for improving dopamine function, for deliberate cold exposure, for fitness, for learning and neuroplasticity, and much more. To sign up for the newsletter, simply go to hubermanlab.com,

go to the menu tab, scroll down to newsletter and supply your email. Again, the newsletter is completely zero cost and I want to emphasize that we do not share your email with anybody. Thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion with Dr. and Professor Mark Desposito. And last but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.