♪ Engelbert's Rules ♪
If your bingo has ads in it, that's not a bingo. If it doesn't have the coolest tournaments, mini games, and the most breathtaking design, nope, not a bingo. If your bingo moment makes you feel so excited that you just want to burst in joy and scream out loud, bingo! Sorry. So you're playing Bingo Blitz? Now that's a bingo. Discover a world of excitement with Bingo Blitz, the number one free bingo game. Download Bingo Blitz and play for free. Now that's a bingo.
Today, on Something You Should Know: Why major storms get named, and what happens to those names afterwards. Then, the interesting questions about what makes us uniquely human. Questions like: Is the primary function of sex in humans to reproduce? Because statistically, the answer appears to be very much no. Is that unique to humans? And the answer turns out to be, well, no, because loads of animals have non-reproductive sex.
Also, what should and should not go down your garbage disposal, and the power of language and how the language you speak affects you. For instance: So some languages only have the words for black, white and red. They don't actually have words for other colours. Now, if you don't have a word for blue or green or yellow, does that impact the way you perceive blue, green and yellow? And if you don't have a word for embarrassment or shame, does that mean you don't really feel embarrassment or shame?
All this today on Something You Should Know. How many times have you heard me say, if you need to hire, you need Indeed? Still, I know many employers will go it alone searching to find people on their own, which is time-consuming, sometimes frustrating, and unnecessary. Instead of searching for the right person, match the right person to the job with Indeed.
Indeed is your matching and hiring platform with over 350 million global monthly visitors, according to Indeed data, and a matching engine that helps you find quality candidates fast. But it's not just about the speed. 93% of employers agree that Indeed delivers the highest quality matches compared to other job sites, according to a recent Indeed survey.
Look, if you're hiring, and I've been in that situation many times, what I love about Indeed is it's a process, a well-honed process. It takes the whole hit or miss element out of the picture because you don't want that.
Listeners of this show will get a $75 sponsored job credit to get your job more visibility at Indeed.com slash something. Just go to Indeed.com slash something right now and support this show by saying you heard about Indeed on something you should know. Indeed.com slash something. Terms and conditions apply. Need to hire? You need Indeed.
Something you should know. Fascinating Intel. The world's top experts and practical advice you can use in your life. Today, something you should know with Mike Carruthers.
Hi, welcome. This is Something You Should Know. We start today by talking about storms. Unfortunately, over the last several years, we've heard a lot about and many people have experienced some very dangerous storms. And those big storms all get a name. And there's a reason for that.
It can get complicated tracking storms and trying to alert the public using the location of the storm, latitude and longitude. By naming a potentially dangerous storm, it makes it a lot easier for everyone to follow along. Storm naming dates back centuries. In the 19th century, storms were named after the Saints' day on which they hit.
After that, Australians started using female names. The National Hurricane Center in this country jumped on that bandwagon in 1953, and then they introduced male names in 1979. But winter storms didn't start getting names until 2012, and we have the Weather Channel to thank for that.
Alphabetical is still the go-to order for naming storms. And if a storm develops into a monster, like Milton, Ian, Andrew, Katrina, Sandy, then those names are retired forever. And that is something you should know. Here is something I bet you've thought about from time to time, and that is, are we humans really exceptional from other creatures on Earth, or are we just different?
There are many things that people assume are uniquely human that turn out not to be. But there are some interesting things about humans that are unique to us that you may not have even considered. This conversation that you're about to hear is going to make you think differently about being a member of the human race. My guest is Adam Rutherford. He is a science writer and broadcaster and author of the book,
The Book of Humans: A Brief History of Culture, Sex, War, and the Evolution of Us. Hey Adam, welcome to Something You Should Know. Thank you Mike, it's very nice to be on your show. So I think everyone has their own thoughts about how humans are exceptional, how we're similar or different to other animals on Earth. So if you would, just pick an example of something most of us believe about being human,
and drill down into what you discovered that would be really surprising. So for example, right, sex is a really interesting example for humans because if you ask any high school student what is the primary function of sex, they will say it is to reproduce, right? Sex is for reproduction and that is true for any sexual organism. Until you look at the stats for humans, right,
where if you begin to account for the number of times that humans have sexual intercourse, male-female sexual intercourse, that actually results in a pregnancy, the answer is that it's statistically insignificant.
Right. It's, you know, it's point zero one percent or something just completely off off the statistical significant charts. So you say, well, what is the primary function of sex? It's to reproduce. But actually, the amount of reproduction that happens as a result of having sex is is almost immeasurable. And then if you add on top of that.
all of the sexual acts that can't result in a pregnancy, then the number just vanishes. And so you have to re-ask the question, well, is the primary function of sex in humans to reproduce? Because statistically, the answer appears to be very much no.
Is that unique to humans? Is this behaviour, which is a sort of extraction from biological evolution, is this something that only humans do? And almost every time asking these types of questions, the answer turns out to be, well, no, because loads of animals have non-reproductive sex. And one of the functions of sex is reproduction.
But there are tons of other purposes of sex, which means that every sexual behavior that we see and enjoy in humans, we also see in other animal species. Every single one that you, the listener, are currently thinking of, but I won't go into detail. I'll leave that up to your imagination. Well, that's such a great example because, I mean, you're right. Ask anybody. The purpose of sex, it's to make babies. And yet there's an awful lot of sex there.
That has nothing to do with making a baby. But what you said about other animals, that does surprise me because I've always thought that it was only humans that had non-reproductive sex before.
But you're saying other animals do too. That's fascinating. Yeah, yeah. And so whilst looking at these sort of uniqueness theories, and lots of people over the years have had successful careers researching and writing popular books about these uniqueness theories. You know, here is the thing that makes humans different from other species.
And pretty much every single one turns out to be not nearly as unique or not nearly as uniquely human as previously thought. So, you know, another obvious one is communications. Like, what are we doing now? We are speaking to each other in a lexicon that has previously agreed over thousands of generations to the extent that this complex vocal anatomy is producing sound waves into this microphone that is being transmitted across the world.
the Atlantic to you and it's feeding into your eardrums and you can understand what it is that I'm saying. Right? That's amazing. That's an amazing thing. But animal communication is something that happens in all animal species, perhaps not to the same degree of sophistication. But Darwin had this phrase in The Descent of Man where he talks about
difference in scale, but not in type, right? That, I think, is much more where humans are on that sort of spectrum. We definitely have the most sophisticated communications of any animal that we are currently aware of. But animals are communicating all the time in super sophisticated ways. And it's only really in the last few decades that we've begun to listen. So again, not nearly as unique as we used to think we were.
One thing that I've heard is uniquely human that I want you to comment on is our human ability to be cruel for the sake of being cruel, that human beings hurt and kill just for the heck of it kind of thing, and that other species don't do that. True?
I think there is some truth in that. So we rely, as a sort of ultra-empirical view of nature, which I think we're obliged to take as scientists, even though it's kind of dull,
that the universe is indifferent. Evolution is absolutely indifferent to suffering apart from humans. And so maybe it is the case that we have the capacity to be cruel or to be sympathetic and empathetic in a way that other animals can't because the rest of nature isn't cruel. We cannot say that any aspect of nature
of behavior by non-human animals is cruel, what we can say is that they are indifferent to suffering. Well, again, what you just pointed out is so interesting because, well, here's the example. You see these videos on TV from time to time from some closed circuit camera in New York or wherever, where some thug goes up to somebody at random and just hits them, smacks them in the face.
You don't see that with other animals. I mean, it doesn't seem like you do. I've never seen it where people just go hit people, cause suffering for no reason other than to seemingly just to do it. But as you just pointed out, on the other hand...
Usually, you know, people rush to that person's rescue. We are empathetic and sympathetic, and you don't see that in other animals too often either, that they're somewhat indifferent. They just go on about their day. So whereas humans can be cruel, we can also be kind.
Yes, I think that's right. And the kindness comes from our sociality and maybe the cruelness does as well, our competitiveness and our warlike tendencies that we form tribes. War and violence is another way of sort of assessing human uniqueness theories because...
We have gone to war and we have been at war pretty much continuously for the last at least 10,000 years or so. Do we see other examples of territorial formalised battles in other organisms? And the answer is mostly not. We do see hints of it in chimpanzees. The behaviour, as far as we can tell, looks quite similar to the sort of systematic violence and territorial challenge, territorial takeovers in some countries.
some examples that is reminiscent of what we would describe as war. The use of weapons, you might think, well, that's a very human characteristic and we've honed our technology to weapons of terrifying, destructive capabilities. But lots of organisms use tools in defence and in
And that's not just primates. That goes all the way down to creatures that we've been, you know, evolutionarily separated from for literally millions, tens, hundreds of millions of years. Things like crabs, pom-pom crabs who use...
torn up anemones held in their claws to fight with other crabs for territory. I mean, that particular example I'm very fond of because I'm sure it's terrifying if you're another crab, but they look like cheerleaders to us because they, and we refer to them as pom-pom crabs rather than boxer crabs because they sort of
move around with these little fluffy balls. But nevertheless, you know, it's an example of using something available in their environment as a weapon. And that doesn't imply that there is shared ancestry between those crabs doing that and us.
But it does imply quite strongly that using things available around in your environment as weapons of war or as weapons of attack is something that is very, very cross-species and part of the lexicon of human evolution. When you look at other species, like watch bees or watch a herd of whatever,
There seems to be an order. There seems to be, you know, rules that they're following. Whereas humans have rules and people follow them, but there are a lot of people who don't follow them. They fall outside the rules and they break the rules. And I wonder, is that uniquely human? Because other species seem to have more of a, everybody does their job and everybody seems to understand what the rules are.
I don't know about that. I mean, yeah, I guess. We have formulated rules which are determined as socially constructed ideas. They are things that we agree by consensus are...
this is how you should behave in society. And, you know, one of the most obvious ones is thou shalt not kill, which is not a specifically Christian or Old Testament Ten Commandment. Almost every culture has a version of thou shalt not kill. And how are we doing on that as a species? Well, you know, not great. I feel like that if Jesus were to come back today, he'd probably say, do I need to clarify what I meant by thou shalt not kill? Because you guys don't appear to be doing very well at it.
But those rules are...
are determined by our cultural and social interactions. We agree as societies that these are the ways that we should behave and those change over time. They change, some of them change very slowly, some of them change incredibly quickly. The acceptance of a seemingly universal human characteristics such as homosexuality, male-male and female-female, appears to have existed in every society for as long as we've had humans.
but its acceptance in society has changed significantly over time. But it's happened very quickly in the last 10 or 15 years, at least in this country. I mean, I'm in London. In a way that, you know, my children aged 16 and 18 find it absolutely baffling. My point being that the rules that we impose are done by consensus and they're subject to our cultural norms and the way our culture changes. Whereas animals...
They don't tend to have rules like that, as far as we're aware. I mean, that may not be the case in some social animals. I'm not sure. Yeah.
But yeah, that is a cultural difference, which I think is not uniquely human, but looks like a very human trait. We're talking about what it is that makes us uniquely human. And my guest is Adam Rutherford. He's author of The Book of Humans, A Brief History of Culture, Sex, War, and the Evolution of Us.
You know, it's always fun to tell people about something new you've discovered, like some great new show to watch on TV or some great new song. Well, lately I've been telling people about Mint Mobile because they offer premium wireless service for just $15 a month. And I love to tell people about this because, well, look, if you pay your mobile phone bill every month, I know it's not $15.
So I'm now with Mint Mobile, and the service is great. So say goodbye to your overpriced wireless plan and switch to Mint Mobile. All plans come with high-speed data and unlimited talk and text, delivered on the nation's largest 5G network. You can use your own phone with any Mint Mobile plan and bring your phone number along with all your existing contacts.
To get this new customer offer and your new 3-month premium wireless plan for just $15 a month, go to mintmobile.com slash something. That's mintmobile.com slash something. Cut your wireless bill to $15 a month at mintmobile.com slash something.
$45 upfront payment required, equivalent to $15 a month. New customers on first three-month plan only. Speed slower above 40 gigabytes on unlimited plan. Additional tax fees and restrictions apply. See Mint Mobile for details.
It is great how the internet has leveled the playing field and now anyone can sell anything anywhere. What gets a little complicated are all the details of setting up your online store and making sure everything runs flawlessly, which is why you need Shopify.
In fact, that's the sound of another sale on Shopify. Shopify is the commerce platform that's revolutionizing millions of businesses worldwide. Whether you're selling roller skates or roofing tiles, Shopify simplifies selling online and in person so you can focus on successfully growing your business.
Shopify covers every sales channel, from an in-person POS system to an all-in-one e-commerce platform. It even lets you sell across social media marketplaces like TikTok, Facebook, and Instagram, and they cover all that for you. Shopify gives you complete control over your business with state-of-the-art tools without you having to learn any new skills in design or code or anything like that.
As someone who has worked with e-commerce, I know it can get a little technical and overwhelming. But Shopify makes it easy. And easy is good. And you'll like this. You can test it, put it through its paces for a dollar. Sign up for a $1 per month trial period at shopify.com slash S-Y-S-K, all lowercase. Go to shopify.com slash S-Y-S-K to grow your business, no matter what stage you're in.
So Adam, I think whenever you talk about what makes us uniquely human, you have to consider that it's our brain. That we have a very advanced brain that's able to think. The fact that we can actually sit and talk about what makes us human is what makes us human. Because other species...
can't do that. They can't examine things and discuss things like we do. And that is, in fact, what makes us uniquely human.
Well, I think there's a foundation of truth in that, but I think we have to be more specific than just referring to it as our brain, because I think there is a uniqueness theory that is emerging from this type of work, and this is the work I write about and talk about, but there's lots of researchers thinking about this. And it's not specifically our brains. We don't have the biggest brains nor the most complex brains, right?
And we sort of are slightly dependent on how we measure intelligence or cognitive abilities, which is a very sort of human-centric, anthropocentric type of idea. But I think it's more to do with...
the fact that we are teachers, that the way that we transmit knowledge between generations and indeed with our peers from me to you and you to me and everyone you communicate with on a day-to-day basis, you are voluntarily sharing information which does not necessarily help your survival.
And that type of cultural transmission of knowledge and accumulation of knowledge, that I think is uniquely human. And I think it's not just uniquely human, but I think our ability to do that marks...
the transition from this earlier version of humans into the modern version of humans, which happens at some point, and by point I mean, you know, over the course of several thousand years, but at something happens in the last hundred thousand years where we see an earlier version where information is not retained in a longitudinal way and afterwards
It is. Now we think, because people in my lab have modelled this based on the input of things like the emergence of art and the emergence of sophisticated technologies all around the world. And what we think is the key factor is the size of populations. So at a point where our populations begin to explode in various places around the world in the last 100,000 years or so, we suddenly see...
the emergence of things like cave art or sophisticated tools that didn't exist beforehand. We are a species of experts in a way that no other organism is. So if you want to learn about human evolution, you come and talk to me. If I want to learn about, I don't know, Shakespeare, I'll go to a Shakespeare expert. If I want my car repaired, I'll go to the mechanic. I'll go take the car into the shop because I can't do that.
No other species has that type of specialisation. And you can imagine in an evolutionary context that this type of behaviour where people become experts and transmit their expertise into their social group, in a small population, that information can be lost over time. But in a big population, if you can convey it to a larger audience, then in the era of pre-writing...
then some of those ideas begin to stick. And so it is the accumulation of knowledge over generational time which I think makes us teachers, and I think that's what makes us unique. But we couldn't do any of that without our brain, our very sophisticated human brain. Yeah, that is definitely true. I mean, that is absolutely true. We've got a very, very large cerebral cortex and an ability to retain and convey information.
But I think really, and I don't want to sound like mystical or sort of pan-psychic about this, it's not just our individual brains, it is the space between our brains which allows this to happen. It's that we need our brains, not we need the interaction between my brain and your brain for that knowledge to become permanent. What else would people find interesting because it is uniquely human or interesting
interesting because we think it's uniquely human, but it really is not. Well, I love talking about tools, actually. And it was Darwin who suggested that we were uniquely tool users. And this was sort of definitionally human because, you know, what are we doing right now? I've got a computer in front of me with a microphone, my phone's there, and there's a pair of scissors on the table. And we've been tool users for millions of years, long before Homo sapiens actually existed.
our evolutionary ancestors were using stone tools and wooden tools. We literally name eras after the type of tools that were being used, the Neolithic, the new Stone Age.
And so you go, well, is it just humans? You know, is it is tools the things that make us humans? And the answer to that is absolutely not. And we see tool use in hundreds of species across many different types of of organisms ranging from birds. So, for example, the corvids. So those are like crows, right?
We know that they use sticks to fish out grubs or they have incredible cognitive abilities to use tools. We know that orangutans sharpen sticks and use them to spearfish. Chimpanzees also sharpen sticks and use them to sort of kebab sleeping bush babies in trees.
And one of the very best, my favorite example of tool use is specifically about a subset of tools, which is fire, right? Humans have really learned to control fire and use that as a tool to build fire.
not just the modern world, but to cook foods, which completely changed our diet and our ability to move around the planet, especially to move to colder areas. So maybe it was fire that is uniquely human. Well, it turns out, absolutely not. There are three species of raptor birds in Australia that in the last couple of years, 2019 it was, the paper was published, where they were observed hanging around in trees,
where bushfires had started and they pick up a stick that was burning in their claws and fly off with a burning stick and fly over natural fire barriers such as roads or rivers and drop the stick in dry areas of brush and start a new fire and then they go and sit in a tree and wait and as the fire spreads the
the little critters, the animals that are running away from being burnt to death, run out and form a buffet, a moving buffet for these birds to pick off and eat. So we've got bird species that we are more than 200 million years distantly evolutionarily related to, also having a command of fire in order to survive.
which I just think is so, so magical. And I think it's one of those things that you think, God, that's so, you know, our ability to control fire has built the modern world. We are so dependent on our ability to control fire. And that must be uniquely human. Well, it turns out the birds have been doing it for at least 20,
a hundred million years longer than humans have ever existed. Well, I really like having conversations like this. They get me to think about a topic in a very different way, and you've certainly accomplished that today. I've been talking with Adam Rutherford. He's a science writer and broadcaster in the UK, and he is author of the book, The Book of Humans, A Brief History of Culture, Sex, War, and the Evolution of Us.
And there's a link to his book at Amazon in the show notes. Great having you on. Thanks, Adam. Oh, thank you. I mean, if I can make people think differently about things, then my job is done. As a podcast network, our first priority has always been audio and the stories we're able to share with you. But at Realm, we also sell some pretty cool merch. And organizing that was made both possible and easy with Shopify.
When you think about successful businesses like Allo or Allbirds or Skims, an often overlooked secret is the business behind the business that makes selling and for shoppers buying simple. For millions of businesses, that business is Shopify. That's because nobody does selling better than Shopify. It's the home of the number one checkout on the planet. And the not so secret secret that's definitely worth talking about is that ShopPay boosts conversions up to 50 percent.
That's more happy customers and way more sales going. If you're hoping to grow your business, your commerce platform better be ready to sell wherever your customers are scrolling or strolling on the web, in your store, in their feed and everywhere in between. Businesses that sell more sell on Shopify. Upgrade your business and get the same checkout we use with Shopify. Sign up for your $1 per month trial period at shopify.com slash realm, all lowercase.
Go to shopify.com slash realm to upgrade your selling today. Shopify.com slash realm.
Meaningless stuff. Why do we gift so much meaningless stuff? Tired of generic gifts and hollow trends? Shutterfly allows you to create meaningful photo gifts for your family and friends, whether it's a cozy fleece blanket for grandma, a stunning canvas print for mom, or a mug to make dad smile. Enjoy 40% off with code MAKE40 at Shutterfly.com and make something meaningful this year. See site for more details.
The world is full of languages. We use words to communicate. And yes, we also have gestures and tone of voice and facial expressions. But at the heart of our communication are words, whether it's English words or any other language.
And it makes you wonder, where did language come from? And how did people communicate before language existed? How does language evolve into what it is?
It's interesting stuff, and here to discuss it is Stephen Mithen. He's a professor of early prehistory at the University of Reading, an esteemed archaeologist, and author of over 200 articles and books. His latest book is called The Language Puzzle, piecing together the six million year story of how words evolved. Hi Stephen, welcome to Something You Should Know. Hello Mike, it's great to be here.
So do we know, can you explain like when talking started and what did people do before they started talking? Did they grunt or I mean, how did it all happen? Well, that's the big question. I mean,
as you say million-dollar questions what does what does talking mean because even when we talk to each other we use loads of gestures and body language and that's communication all sorts of information and you know what we're thinking in our emotional states
and we see that in all sorts of animals around the world you know i mean insects communicate with each other some people say plants communicate but what you're talking about is the foundations of the language that we have as human beings now our close living relatives are the chimpanzees we shared an ancestor of them between six and eight million years ago and we say see they've got a pretty developed form of both vocal and gestural communication they don't use words
but they use a whole variety of grunts and chatters and barks and so forth which are communicating something normally it's information about how they're feeling sometimes it's warnings to each other sometimes it's their anger their emotions and they use gesture as well and we've got a guest our common ancestor with those chimpanzees that lived
about six million years ago we think that's by comparison comparing our dna probably communicated in the same way and that our language today our spoken language with its words and its grammar and so forth evolved from that that basis over the last six million years so when chimpanzees make those noises and grunt and and bark and whatever they do is it a language you can translate like oh that grunt means
Tree. Not in that detail. We often refer to them as holistic communication because they tend to make a whole sequence of sounds, barks and grunts, varying in their pitch and their tone and so forth. And together, that phrase normally means something like,
"Beware, there's a snake in the grass." Now, we don't know if it means "Beware, there's a snake in the grass" or means "Run away and hide" or "I'm scared of that, what's moving." We can't translate it literally, but the impact of it is as if we were saying, "Watch out, there's a snake in the grass." And the chimpanzees have these, chimpanzees and other primates as well, they have these phrases that have that impact on others.
But it's not as if one bit of their sound phrase relates to one, what we'd think of one word or one entity. It's like a holistic phrase. And all the chimpanzees know exactly what he's talking about. Yeah, broadly. Yeah. I mean, a lot of it seems to be instinctive, not only hearing them, but also when making those sounds. There's been some great work with vervet monkeys, and you can see that the juveniles, they're
They're sort of learning this language, but some of it is instinctive. So whereas the adults, if they see an eagle or something like that, they'll have an alarm call for it. Now the young ones might make that alarm call when they see perhaps some leaves floating off the trees. So they've sort of got most of it genetically inherited, but they're still doing a little bit of learning about how these phrases work.
but they're really limited you know they only have a small amount of these phrases and they can't sort of vary them so you can't they can't sort of a monkey can't sort of say uh has an alarm call for an eagle
Then switch it to some other creature that it's never seen before They're very limited in there in their range very different like human language where we're able to make up sentences about Basically anything we like and we mix words together remix meanings together So that's what humans did initially when they started communicating with each other much like the chimpanzees. I
We think so, yeah. When we say humans, of course, we mean our sort of more ape-like ancestors that we'd call the earliest humans. These are the Australopithecines living between six and two million years ago, and then the earliest members of our homogeneous Homo habilis and then Homo erectus lived up to about a million years or so ago.
Well, it sure makes you wonder, like, so how did it happen where somebody somewhere somehow must have said, hey, I got an idea. We're going to call this a rock and everybody's going to call this a rock and you're going to know exactly what we're talking about because we're all going to agree that this is now a rock. Yeah, well, it couldn't have happened like that, could it? Well, probably not. It just couldn't happen like that. And, you know, that was one of the great dilemmas that
the great thinkers of the in the enlightenment period um they couldn't resolve this because they thought well if we're going to talk we've got to have the ideas on our head to talk with
But then those ideas really come from the words that we use. Can you have ideas without words? Well, you can, but they're probably quite limited. So how all the language thing got going is the really big question. Now, probably in my view and a view of many others as well, the most obvious way it would get going would by using words like onomatopoeias. If I mimic a bird call, for instance, it's not a word for a bird.
but that word can eventually stand for the word for the bird. And I might make a movement or make a sound which doesn't mimic the sound of the bird or an animal, but maybe the way it moves, you know, a low, heavy grunting sound for what an elephant is like. So these onomatopoeias are what we call iconic words or words that capture the senses of something moving. That's probably the earliest words because
we can say those and somebody else can have a good guess as what they mean because they're having the same sensations as us when they're seeing the same thing or looking at it. And these are called iconic words and they're still very pervasive in our language today. In fact,
When children are acquiring language, most of the early words they learn are what we call iconic words. They have this sense of the object within them, rather than being entirely arbitrary, like many of our words, that most of our words are, we use, especially when they're adults. And those words would be words like?
Well, like, for instance, you know, a dog, we call a dog in English or we call it a hund in German or chien in French. It's a purely arbitrary word for the what you're looking at. You know, the word dog doesn't suggest it's got four legs or tail or barks, nor does the word hund.
But if I would said to you a word like balloon. Now, when I say balloon, my mouth becomes rather round and I make these rounded sounds of these rounded vowel sounds. You might have quite a good guess if you're looking at various objects, which one of those would fit with the word balloon.
as opposed to another word which might have lots of little short, sharp, spiky vowels. You might guess that relates to some small spiky thing. Psychologists have tested these with humans. They've given them shapes of different types, and then they've given them these types of words which have these different vowel and consonant sounds, and so they've matched them up.
And generally, words that have low back vowels with O's and OO's, people associate those with round shapes, obviously. And words that have very sharp edges like click, clack, clack,
they'd be associated with sharp things. Now that makes sense because it connects the sounds we're saying and what we're seeing. And it was probably that sort of process that got words going in the first place. And, you know, my guess is the earliest languages depended on these, what we call iconic words or sound symbolic words. And it was rather later in time, once people got used to that sort of language,
And when people began to need more words, the arbitrary words were invented and came to be learned and could be remembered and recalled. And who did that? And when did that happen? I reckon that's happening at around, probably at around half a million years ago. You see, around that time,
we have our ancestors, they evolved from Homo erectus to a rather larger brain species, one that we call Homo heidelbergensis, that we think is the ancestor of both the Neanderthals and modern humans.
Now we can see a lot of behavioural change with those species. They're the first ones that start using fire really, you know, on a large and regular scale. They're making much more sophisticated stone tools and they've particularly got larger brains.
Now, it's likely that the complexity of the lifestyles by that time is simply needing more words than those iconic words can provide. And they've also got a larger brain with a larger memory capacity. So I think at that time, the way language is evolving, we've got the increasing presence of
entirely arbitrary words. And that's when language really begins to flourish and begins to extend, not just one language, but into the multiple languages like we have today. So I'd put that key change at about half a million years ago. I find it interesting, like, so British English and American English, you can come here and do fine, and I could go there and do fine. But there are words that you use that I don't use. And
And words that I use that you don't use. And I wonder what they're so, so similar, but then there's these certain words and phrases that are so, so different. It's partly it's culture. It's social tradition, how it comes. They're quite how they become adopted ultimately goes down to, you know, details of social interactions and transitions and origins, et cetera.
But as you say, you know, there's simple things, aren't there? Like, what would you say? You'd say the sidewalk, I'd say the pavement. The boot of your car, and I'd say the trunk. Yeah.
That's right, and I'd say a flat and you'd say an apartment. Yeah, so these are arbitrary differences in language which don't really have any significant effect on a level of the communication. But we adopt those words because they're the socially accepted way of speaking. I'd be fascinated to know how all these different languages started and why some of them are similar to others.
and some people speak multiple languages. How all that works? From my experience of working with linguists and knowing linguists, people who speak, especially people who speak multiple languages, the more languages you speak, the easier it becomes to learn another language because you sort of intuitively
identifies similarities with the language you speak, you already speak, and you come to recognise those similarities. But you know, one of the really interesting things is whether the language you speak
changes the way you think about the world. That's a really interesting issue about language. We don't use only language to communicate, we use it to think ourselves. We're always turning ideas over in our head. But whether the words we use influence the way we think and see the world is a really big issue that's been discussed by philosophers and psychologists and linguists. I think that's also relevant for the evolution of language.
Because we do in the past, in prehistory, see sudden periods of immense cultural diversity when people start living in very different ways. And my guess is they're perceiving the world quite differently, which probably reflects quite different languages that they're using at that time. Yeah, because you think in your language, right? I don't think people think about that very often. But like if you said...
think of a red flower, you know, I'm thinking the words red flower. Yeah, but what some people have argued is that different languages have different types of colour words. Okay? So, some languages only have the words for black, white and red. They don't actually have words for other colours. Now, if you don't have a word for blue or green or yellow, does that impact the way you perceive blue, green and yellow?
you know do you see do you see and think of them as just ranges of grays or reds and so forth um or if you don't have words for a particular type of emotion like um you know if you don't have a word for embarrassment or shame
Does that mean you don't really feel embarrassment or shame? Or could you not differentiate those particular emotions? So, we have different words for everything, but if you don't have those words, how does that affect your particular thoughts? I think these are really interesting issues about language. So, there are languages that don't have words for green and blue, so what do they say when they see green and blue?
They don't talk about it, I think. They don't label it. I'm not sure what they actually say. I mean, people have done colour tests of these, and they can certainly distinguish them, but they don't have a label for these words. It's like other languages, you know, some languages don't have names for lots of numbers, you know, they may have words for one, two, three, four, but not necessarily have a language where you can
create a number a word for any number you like that we can do in our in english so these are these are um you know really interesting issues about the relationship between language and thought and culture
Well, doesn't that seem odd though? That how could you live in this world and not perceive and want to identify something that was green or especially blue? I mean, the sky is blue. I mean, how can you not have a color for that? Well, you don't necessarily have, you don't necessarily need to label it differently from other colors, perhaps. Anyway, it's been a huge issue in linguistics for, you know, 50 years or so trying to work out why some languages have lots of color words.
and some languages have very few color words. Where does English fall on that spectrum? Do we have... We do pretty well, yeah, we do pretty well. I mean, we have about 11 really big color terms, you know, the primary colors. Then, of course, we have loads of words for very fine distinctions of colors, like...
Crimson or Zillion or aqua and things are that that we very rarely use right and in fact in fact, you know if you said that's aqua I might say no I see that more as sort of turquoise or something like that We don't necessarily have a really clear definition of those sorts of words and even I you know I always arguing in my wife about whether something's blue or green because we seem to see it differently and we seem to label things differently so
We use words as labels, but it doesn't necessarily mean we're always talking about the same thing, does it? Were there times in history where there were big changes in language because of what was going on in the world?
So probably a big change that happened in the race of language evolution was when people discovered agriculture about 10,000 years ago. You know, before that, people were living in small, mobile hunter-gatherer societies. And I suspect many of those hunter-gatherer groups had quite specific languages that had been pretty...
very slowly changing, if at all, for hundreds, maybe thousands of years. But when you start getting the big farming communities, then the first towns and the first ancient civilizations like in ancient Mesopotamia, that must have been a time when land began to change very, very rapidly because of just the scale of the community and the diversity of activities. And of course, that's also at the time that writing is invented for the first time.
And that marked a big change in language evolution because now people are for the first time are writing down eventually words and what people are saying. And that sort of freezes language in a way. So today spoken language tends to change at a much quicker rate than written language. You know, when we write, we tend to have to use all the grammatical rules and so forth. When we speak, we very rarely use all of those. So
There's a difference between spoken and written language at the rate at which they change.
And the rate of both of those depends on the size of the speech community and its economy and its diversity and so forth. This is all related to the era of sociolinguistics, which tries to understand the relationship between language and language change and the nature of society. You know what I wonder is, if I were to travel back in time, say just 30 years ago, 40 years ago, and talked with my grandmother,
Would the language seem off just in 40 years, or does it not change that fast? I'm sure you could understand each other and have good conversation. But of course, the particular words you'd use would be quite different. Imagine your grandmother would be talking about a gramophone or a wireless set.
And sure here you would have names for all sorts of tools and implements that you have no idea what they are, how they use.
And equally, if you talk to your, well, if I talk to my grandchildren now, they'll come out with words that I'm not quite sure what the meaning of those are because of the way they're using. They'll have lots of words about high tech and games and online stuff that I don't understand at all. So I think language does change really rapidly, not becomes unintelligible, but just because society and technology is changing so much.
Well, it's quite a story and quite a journey from the grunts and groans of early times to
The language, the very adaptable language that we have today. I appreciate you coming on and telling that story. Stephen Mithen has been my guest. He is a professor of early prehistory at the University of Reading, an esteemed archaeologist and author of over 200 articles and books. His latest book is called The Language Puzzle, piecing together the six million year story of how words evolved. There's a link to his book at Amazon in the show notes.
Appreciate you coming on. Thank you for being here, Stephen. Well, thank you, Mike. It's been great talking with you. A common kitchen debate you've heard and maybe been involved in is what can and cannot go down the garbage disposal. You may have heard that things like chicken bones and eggshells are good to put in the disposal from time to time because they help sharpen the blades. On the other hand, you may have heard that chicken bones and eggshells are the worst thing to put down there.
So, Consumer Reports finally got to the bottom of the debate. Their experts say both eggshells and chicken bones take a long time to decompose, and once they're chopped up, deposits can get stuck in traps and sections of the pipe, causing blockages. It's best to skip bones and shells in your disposal.
And while we're on the topic, other things that should not go down the disposal, according to Consumer Reports, are grease and oil, a large number of vegetable peels, better to throw those in the trash, and anything that is not food. And that is something you should know.
It may seem like a little thing, but the very best way you can support this podcast is to help us grow our audience one person at a time, or maybe two people at a time. So if you share this podcast by using the share function on the platform that you listen to on, you can send an episode of this podcast to someone and they can listen and become another listener. I'm Mike Carruthers. Thanks for listening today to Something You Should Know.
Welcome to the small town of Chinook, where faith runs deep and secrets run deeper. In this new thriller, religion and crime collide when a gruesome murder rocks the isolated Montana community. Everyone is quick to point their fingers at a drug-addicted teenager, but local deputy Ruth Vogel isn't convinced. She suspects connections to a powerful religious group. Enter federal agent V.B. Loro, who has been investigating a local church for possible criminal activity.
The pair form an unlikely partnership to catch the killer, unearthing secrets that leave Ruth torn between her duty to the law, her religious convictions, and her very own family. But something more sinister than murder is afoot, and someone is watching Ruth. Chinook, starring Kelly Marie Tran and Sanaa Lathan. Listen to Chinook wherever you get your podcasts. Contained herein are the heresies of Rudolf Bantwine, erstwhile monk turned traveling medical investigator.
Join me as I study the secrets of the divine plagues and uncover the blasphemous truth that ours is not a loving God and we are not its favored children. The Heresies of Redolf Bantwine, wherever podcasts are available.